Research in Developmental Disabilities 35 (2014) 2644–2650

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Research in Developmental Disabilities

Executive function and attention in young adults with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder – A comparative study Miri Tal Saban a,*, Asher Ornoy b,c, Shula Parush a a b c

School of Occupational Therapy, Hebrew University Hadassah Medical School, Jerusalem, Israel Hebrew University Hadassah Medical School, Jerusalem, Israel Israeli Ministry of Health, Jerusalem, Israel

A R T I C L E I N F O

A B S T R A C T

Article history: Received 16 March 2014 Received in revised form 30 June 2014 Accepted 2 July 2014 Available online

The current research aimed at examining the executive function (EF) of young adults with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) in comparison to young adults without DCD. The study used a randomized cohort (N = 429) of young adults with DCD (n = 135), borderline DCD (n = 149) and control (n = 145), from a previous study. This initial cohort was asked to participate in the current study three to four years later. Twenty-five individuals with DCD (mean age = 24 years, 1 month [SD = 0.88]; 18 males), 30 with borderline DCD (mean age = 24 years, 2 month [SD = 0.98]; 18 males) and 41 without DCD (mean age = 25 years, 2 months [SD = 1.91]; 20 males) participated in this study. Participants completed the BRIEF-A questionnaire, assessing EF abilities and the WURS questionnaire, assessing attention abilities. The DCD and borderline DCD groups had significantly lower EF profiles in comparison with the control group but no significant differences were found between the DCD and borderline DCD groups. While a high percentage of attention problems were found in both DCD groups, the executive functioning profiles remained consistent even when using the attention component as a covariate. The study results suggest that young adults with DCD have EF problems which remain consistent with or without attention difficulties. ß 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) Executive function Attention Young adults

1. Introduction Executive functions (EF) are higher order cognitive processes required to perform novel or complex goal-directed tasks (Barkley, 1997). Research has indicated that EF processes, such as inhibition, anticipating and forward task planning, resisting competing attention demands for postural control, and monitoring and correcting movement errors, are necessary for functional motor control (Livesey, Keen, Rouse, & White, 2006; Roebers & Kauer, 2009). Neuro-imaging studies have shown co-activation of the cerebellum and frontal cortex upon the introduction of novel motor tasks as well as cognitive tasks, suggesting an association between the two areas and leading researchers to postulate that the link between motor

* Corresponding author at: School of Occupational Therapy of Hadassah and the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, PO Box 24026, Mount Scopus, Jerusalem 9124, Israel. E-mail address: [email protected] (M. Tal Saban). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.002 0891-4222/ß 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

M. Tal Saban et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 35 (2014) 2644–2650

2645

coordination and executive functioning, mainly with respect to visuo-spatial working memory and performance speed in motor inhibition tasks, is a result of a shared underlying cerebellar mechanism. This could help explain why many children and adolescents with DCD also have EF deficits (Diamond, 2000; Michel, Roethlisberger, Neuenschwander, & Roberts, 2011; Rigoli, Piek, Kane, & Oosterlaan, 2012). Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) is a severe impairment of motor coordination that significantly interferes with academic achievement and activities of daily living ([DSM-5] APA, 2013). There is evidence of a relationship between motor performance and executive functioning (EF) found in studies examining the functional profile of individuals with DCD (Rigoli et al., 2012), specifically, studies have shown a correlation between DCD and decreased response inhibition (Mandich, Buckolz, & Polatajko, 2002), planning and monitoring motor performance (Livesey et al., 2006; Piek et al., 2004) use of cognitive strategies (Polatajko & Mandich, 2004), and working memory, especially visuo-spatial working memory (Alloway, Rajendran, & Archibald, 2009; Ru Loh, Piek, & Barrett, 2011). Wilson, Ruddock, Smits-Engelsman, Polatajko, and Blank (2013), in performing their meta-analysis of current research regarding the understanding of DCD performance deficits, found pervasive difficulties in EF, especially, working memory, inhibitory control and executive attention (Piek et al., 2004). The majority of existing studies of DCD and EF focus primarily on children (Alloway et al., 2009; Rigoli et al., 2012), with only several studies focusing on the young adult population. Kirby, Sugden, Beveridge, and Edwards (2008), studied 16–25 year-olds with DCD, who reported having motor difficulties since childhood, the participants reported difficulties in motorrelated tasks, such as handwriting, and in executive functions such as time management, memory, organization, planning and decision making. Tal-Saban, Zarka, Grotto, Ornoy, and Parush (2012) assessed the non-academic and academic functioning of young adults with DCD including the use of executive strategies in complex task performance including goal setting, planning and self-evaluation of performance. They found significant differences between the groups, the DCD and borderline DCD groups used less executive strategies than the control group. Rosenblum (2013) evaluated 30 students with DCD and 30 students without DCD in their EF functioning as reflected in the Adult Developmental Coordination Disorder Checklist (ADC). The results showed significant deficits in EF abilities such as attention, planning and organization in young adults with DCD in comparison to the control group. Executive functioning difficulties such as response inhibition, poor sustained attention, working memory, planning, a sense of time and emotional regulation are also characteristic of children and young adults with attention problems (Barkley, 1997; Fischer, Barkley, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2005). It has been shown that children and young adults with attention problems often have coexisting motor coordination difficulties (Fliers et al., 2010), so to, a high percentage of children with motor coordination difficulties also have attention problems (Kaplan, Dewey, Crawford, & Wilson, 2001). Because of this overlap, Kaplan, Crawford, Cantell, Kooistra, and Dewey (2006) suggest the concept of ‘‘Atypical Brain Development (ABD)’’ or ‘‘Minor Neurological Dysfunction,’’ referring to a general underlying impairment of development. ABD emphasizes the interrelatedness of developmental disorders and the common etiology of motor difficulties, ADHD and learning difficulties (Kaplan et al., 2001). In line with this concept, Gillberg and colleagues coined the acronym DAMP – Deficits in Attention Motor control and Perception (Gillberg & Rasmussen, 1982; Hellgren, Gillberg, & Gillberg, 1994). Children with DAMP often have executive function deficits (Gillberg, 2003) in addition to attention, motor and perceptual difficulties, an overlap that can be explained by the hypothesis that DCD and ADHD share an underlying neurocognitive mechanism relating to deficits in executive functioning (Piek et al., 2004). In light of the paucity of research addressing executive functioning in young adults with DCD, the purpose of this study was to examine, in depth, the executive functioning profile of young adults with motor coordination deficits. A secondary purpose was to investigate the prevalence of attention problems amongst young adults with motor coordination difficulties to discern whether the observed executive functioning deficits can be attributed to the attention component.

2. Materials and methods 2.1. Participants The initial study was conducted on a random sample of 2379 young adults from all over Israel, between the ages of 19–25 years (1081 males). The participants had no known birth complications or known psychiatric, physical or neurological deficits. Normative cognition was implied from attendance in regular education frameworks. From this large sample, three smaller study groups were identified based on the cut off scores of the Adolescents and Adults Coordination Questionnaire (AAC-Q; Tal-Saban, Ornoy, Grotto, & Parush, 2012). The DCD group, with 135 subjects at or below the 5th percentile, the borderline DCD group with 149 subjects between the 5th and 15th percentiles and the control groupwith145 individuals who scored within normal range. Groups were matched according to gender and years of education (see Tal-Saban, Zarka, et al. 2012; Tal-Saban, Ornoy, et al. 2012 for more information on the recruitment and group placement of the initial sample). Participants were contacted again between three and four years after initial testing, between the years 2008 and 2009 (mean time from initial contact = 3.58 years; SD = 0.69) and asked to participate in the current follow-up study. Twenty-five individuals from the DCD group (18% of the original sample; mean age = 24 years, 1 month years [SD = 0.88]; 18 males), 30 individuals from the borderline DCD group (20% of the original sample mean age = 24 years, 2 months [SD = 0.98]; 18 males) and 41 individuals from the control group (28% of the original sample; mean age = 25 years, 2 months [SD = 1.91]; 20 males)

2646

M. Tal Saban et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 35 (2014) 2644–2650

agreed to participate. No differences by age, x2 (94, N = 95) = 97.28, p = .39, or gender, x2 (2, N = 95) = 0.43, p = .81, were found among these three groups. Sample size calculations show that this sample has sufficient power for revealing statistically significant effects (Cohen, 1998; Uitenbroek, 1997). Similar sample sizes have been successfully used previously in young adults with DCD research (Kirby et al., 2008; Rasmussen & Gillberg, 2000; Rosenblum, 2013). 2.2. Measures 2.2.1. Behavior rating inventory of executive function-adult version-(BRIEF-A) (Roth, Isquith, & Gioia, 2005) The BRIEF-A Questionnaire is a self-informant report measure which assesses the everyday behavioral manifestations of adult executive function (Roth et al., 2005). It is an ecologically reliable and valid questionnaire, demonstrating high reliability and validity, including internal reliability, test–retest and inter rater reliability (Roth et al., 2005; Waid-Ebbs, Wen, Heaton, Donovan, & Velozo, 2012). The questionnaire includes 75 multiple-choice questions that examine executive functions. The score is based on the results of nine empirically derived clinical scales: (Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control, Self Monitor, Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Task Monitor and Organization of Materials). The sum of the first four scales is referred to as the Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI); the composite score of the five remaining scales is referred to as the Meta-Cognition Index (MI). The Global Executive Composite (GEC) is the overall score of both indices. Responses are scored on a 3-point Likert scale from 1 (never) to 3 (often). Lower scores indicate better executive functioning. Item scores are converted to standard scores and then compared to age and gender norms. 2.2.2. Wender–Utah Rating Scale (WURS) (Ward, Wender, & Reimherr, 1993) The WURS was developed to assist clinicians in the retrospective assessment of the presence and severity of childhood ADHD symptoms in adults (Ward et al., 1993). This questionnaire has been found to distinguish between adults with and without ADHD (McCann, Scheele, Ward, & Roy-Byrne, 2000), supporting the construct validity of the tool. It has acceptable test–retest reliability, internal consistency and concurrent validity (Stein et al., 1995). The scale consists of 61 items assessing a childhood history of ADHD symptoms, of which 25 items are used to differentiate adults with ADHD from a nonpatient comparison group. Each item is measured on a 5-point Likert scale, from 0 (not at all or very slightly) to 4 (very much). A score of 46 or higher, the recommended cut off is indicative of attention problems (Ward et al., 1993). 2.3. Procedure Approval to perform the follow-up study was granted by the Institutional Ethics Committee. Individuals who agreed to participate in the follow-up study were sent the two questionnaires by mail (with a self-addressed return envelope), or by all subjects signed an informed consent form prior to participating in the study. 2.4. Data analysis All statistical calculations were performed by the Statistical Package for Social Sciences for Windows (version 20; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and a type I error rate of 0.05 was used for all statistical analyses. Chi square tests were used to determine differences in descriptive variables between study groups. Group differences were analyzed using multivariate and univariate analyses of variance (MANOVA/ANOVA) and Scheffe´ tests were employed for post hoc analyses. The influence of attention was countered by employing analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with attention as the covariate.

3. Results The participants in the follow-up study were compared to those from the original study who did not participate in the follow up, according to gender and years of education (at the time of the initial study) to assess for possible bias in the current study group. Comparisons were performed separately for each of the three study groups (i.e., DCD, Borderline DCD, Controls) using x2 tests. No group differences were found for any measure with the exception of gender differences, relatively more females participated in the current study (x2 = 3.36; p = 0.01) than in the initial one. There is no missing data in this study. The multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to assess group differences on all thirteen assessment scores, which included the, BRIEF-A (9 clinical scales and 3 index scores) and the WURS total score. Analysis showed a statistically significant difference (F [14,95] = 2.29; p < 0.001; h = 0.284). To examine the source of this difference, the data from each questionnaire was subjected to univariate analyses of variance (ANOVA). Comparisons between the groups revealed significant differences for all scores (Table 1). Scheffe´ posthoc analyses revealed significant differences between the DCD and control group and the borderline DCD and control group, for all questionnaire scores (Table 2). While no differences were found between the DCD and the borderline DCD groups in any of the questionnaires scores (Table 2), the mean of each score revealed that both groups performed consistently lower than the control group across all scores (see Table 2).

M. Tal Saban et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 35 (2014) 2644–2650

2647

Table 1 Means, standard deviations and ANOVA results of group differences in BRIEF-A scores and WURS score. Questionnaire BRIEF-A Inhibit Shift Emotional Control Self-Monitoring Initiate Working Memory Plan/Organize Task Monitoring Organization of Materials Behavioral Regulation Index Metacognitive Index Global Executive Composite WURS (25 q)

DCD (n = 21) Mean (SD)

Borderline (n = 30) Mean (SD)

Control (n = 39) Mean (SD)

F

p

h

53.74 57.67 60.27 53.09 56.09 56.52 54.43 55.86 52.05 59.29 55.71 57.81 59.81

53.43 56.30 57.87 53.10 55.87 55.53 56.80 54.23 53.83 56.97 56.03 57.07 60.83

46.95 49.23 49.82 44.21 46.64 44.46 45.23 45.87 42.92 47.59 44.38 46.13 43.90

8.37 5.63 10.36 11.20 10.47 15.30 11.07 8.65 12.36 13.98 16.07 13.56 11.92

Executive function and attention in young adults with and without developmental coordination disorder--a comparative study.

The current research aimed at examining the executive function (EF) of young adults with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) in comparison to yo...
239KB Sizes 0 Downloads 5 Views