PHB-10506; No of Pages 7 Physiology & Behavior xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Physiology & Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/phb

3Q6

Chunhui Chen a,f,1, Chang Liu b,1, Chuansheng Chen c, Robert Moyzis d,e, Wen Chen a,f, Qi Dong a,f,⁎

O

F

2

Genetic variations in the serotoninergic system and environmental factors contribute to aggressive behavior in Chinese adolescents

4Q7 5 6 7Q8 8 9 10

a

1 1

H I G H L I G H T S

P

R O

c

a r t i c l e

25 26 27 28 29

Keywords: Serotonin Aggressive behavior Polygenicity Heritability

i n f o

a b s t r a c t

48

30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45

viewed as a public health issue. Identifying environmental and genetic risk factors is an important first step in preventing violence and has been the focus of decades of research. Behavior genetic studies estimated the heritability of aggressive behavior to be about 50% [2,3]. Recent molecular genetic studies revealed that many genes are associated with aggression, including: (1) sex steroid-related genes; (2) serotonin- (5-HT) related genes; (3) dopamine-related genes; and (4) a variety of other genes (for reviews, see [4–7]). Of all these genes, serotonin-related genes were the most commonly studied [6,8,9], although the results have been inconsistent. Generally speaking, genotypes associated with lower levels

54

R

R N C O

49 47 46

Aggressive behavior is a major public health problem worldwide and has been associated with many gene variants, especially those related to the serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) system, and environmental factors. However, the overall contribution of serotonin-related genes to aggressive behavior is not well understood. With a sample of 478 healthy Chinese volunteers, this study investigated the relation between aggressive behavior and genetic variations of the serotoninergic system (as characterized by 129 representative polymorphisms) interacting with environmental factors (parental warmth and acceptance; stressful life events). We adopted a system-level approach to identify SNPs and environmental factors associated with aggressive behavior, and estimated their overall contribution to aggressive behavior using multiple regression, which was then verified by permutation analysis. We identified 12 SNPs that made statistically significant contributions to aggressive behavior. Next, main effects, interactions among these SNPs, and interactions between these SNPs and environmental factors were assessed using multiple regression. The final model accounted for approximately 19% of the variance for aggressive behavior. Permutation analysis confirmed that the probability of obtaining these findings by chance was low (p = 0.045, permuted for 1000 times). These results showed that genetic variations in the serotoninergic system, combined with environmental risk factors, made a moderate contribution to individual differences in aggressive behavior among a healthy population sample. © 2014 Published by Elsevier Inc.

C

Article history: Received 4 May 2014 Received in revised form 26 June 2014 Accepted 30 September 2014 Available online xxxx

T

1 9 1 8 20 21 22 23 24

50

1. Introduction

51 Q12

According to the World Health Organization [1], the number of people dying in interpersonal conflicts was almost twice the number of war victims in 2002 and aggressive behavior has been increasingly

52 53

Q9 Q10 Q11

E

D

• With 478 healthy Chinese, we found that 12 serotonin-related SNPs were associated with aggression. • HTR2C interacts with HTR5A as well as stressful life event on aggression. • Gene, environment and their interactions together accounted for 1/5 of aggression variation.

U

12 13 14 15 16 17

State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA d Department of Biological Chemistry, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA e Institute of Genomics and Bioinformatics, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA f Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China b

E

1

⁎ Corresponding author at: State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China. Tel.: +86 10 5880 7615. E-mail address: [email protected] (Q. Dong). 1 These authors contributed equally to this work.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.09.005 0031-9384/© 2014 Published by Elsevier Inc.

Please cite this article as: Chen C, et al, Genetic variations in the serotoninergic system and environmental factors contribute to aggressive behavior in Chinese adolescents, Physiol Behav (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.09.005

55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64

C. Chen et al. / Physiology & Behavior xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

127

2.2.1. Aggression Questionnaire Aggression Questionnaire (AQ) [35] is a 34-item scale that measures aggression in five aspects: physical aggression, verbal aggression, anger, hostility, and indirect aggression. Sample items include “at times I feel like a bomb ready to explode” and “I may hit someone if he or she provokes me”. Participants rated each item on a 5-point scale, 1 = “Disagree strongly” to 5 = “Agree strongly”. The total score of all items was used for analysis.

128 129

2.2.2. Parental Warmth and Acceptance Scale Parental Warmth and Acceptance Scale (PWAS) [36] measures perceived parental warmth with 11 items, such as “My parents really understand me” and “My parents like me the way I am; they don't try to ‘make me over’ into someone else”. Participants rated each item on a 6-point scale, 1 = “Disagree strongly” to 6 = “Agree strongly”. The total score of all items was used for analysis.

136 137

R O

O

F

2.2. Behavioral measurements

C

E

R

R

O

C

N

2. Materials and methods

114

2.1. Participants

115 116

Four hundred and eighty healthy Chinese college students (mean age = 19.9 years, SD = 0.9) were recruited from Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China. They had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and had no history of neurological or psychiatric problems according to self-report. None of them were identified to have alcohol or nicotine dependence according to the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test [33] and the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence [34]. Two participants were excluded because of poor genotyping results. A written consent form was obtained from each subject after a full explanation of the study procedure. This study was approved by the IRB of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning at Beijing Normal University, China.

117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126

U

113

130 131 132 133 134 135

138 139 140 141 142 143

2.3. Genetic analysis

163

2.3.1. Gene selection We selected 129 polymorphisms (127 SNPs and 2 VNTR polymorphisms) in the 24 known major genes distributed across the synthesis, degradation, transporter, and receptor subsystems of the 5-HT system. Tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH) is the main enzyme involved in 5-HT synthesis, so we included two TPH genes (TPH1 and TPH2, with three SNPs each). For the degradation subsystem, released 5-HT is directly broken down at the synapse into inactive metabolites by MAO (including MAOA and MAOB). We included monoamine oxidase genes (MAOA, with 5 SNPs and 1 VNTR, and MAOB with 3 SNPs). The 5-HT transporter

164

Table 1 Means, standard deviations, Cronbach's alpha coefficients, and inter-scale correlations.

t1:1 Q1 Q2 t1:2

P

2.2.3. Stressful life events This scale was adapted from similar measures used in Wills, Vaccaro, and McNamara [37]. The scale has been used with cross-cultural samples including Chinese [36]. It lists 24 possible stressful events such as the death of a relative, not passing an examination, and parents getting divorced. Participants in this study had to indicate whether they experienced each event or not during early childhood (primary school years), early adolescence (secondary school years), and within the past two years (i.e., college years for this sample of college sophomores). The stressful events were counted separately for the three periods. The total score of the stressful life events in three periods was used for analysis. All scales were translated from English to Chinese by a team consisting of Chinese–English bilinguals and native English and Chinese speakers and double-checked with forward and backward translation. All scales had good reliability (Table 1) in this study. Reliability (or internal consistency) for stressful life events was not calculated because stressful life events are assumed to be relatively independent of one another, with no underlying latent factors, and only its cumulative effects are evident.

T

or activity of serotonin or its metabolites were associated with increased levels of aggressive behavior and impulsivity [7,8], but exceptions were 67 also often reported. 68 Tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH) is the rate-limiting enzyme for 69 brain serotonin synthesis. TPH1 and TPH2 genes have been linked 70 to impulsivity, aggressive behavior, and suicide [4,8]. MAOA and 71 MAOB are enzymes that degrade serotonin and other neurotransmit72 ters, and the MAOA gene was once called the “warrior gene”. However, 73 some studies found that low MAOA activity (thus high serotonin level) 74 was associated with low aggression [10], but others reported no associ75 ation [11,12] or reversed association [13]. The serotonin transporter 76 pumps serotonin from the synaptic cleft back to presynaptic neu77 rons. Knocking out the serotonin transporter gene in mice (thus in78 creasing serotonin level in the synaptic cleft) resulted in reduced 79 aggressive behavior [14]. The S allele of the well-studied 5HTTLPR 80 polymorphism (resulting in higher levels of serotonin) was also re81 ported to be associated with decreased aggressive behavior [15], 82 but others reported increased aggressive behavior [16–18]. For sero83 Q13 tonin receptor genes, 5-HT1A/1B receptors were extensively studied, 84 with positive results in some studies [19–21], but negative results in 85 others [22–25]. 86 There may be several reasons for these inconsistencies. One most 87 likely reason is polygenicity. All these genes mentioned above, as well 88 as other genes, may contribute to aggressive behavior, each with a 89 small individual effect. As early as 1918, Fisher proposed this polygenic 90 model that combined many genes of small effects to yield the continu91 ous variation for most quantitative traits [26]. Recently, some studies 92 have successfully applied the polygenic model by combining effects 93 from alleles throughout the whole genome [27] or effects of genes with94 in a biochemical pathway [28,29]. Since the serotonin-related genes 95 exert their effects at different stages of the serotonin pathway, their 96 effects may be cumulative and/or interactive [30]. Another possible rea97 son is that environmental factors are likely to make direct contributions 98 to behavioral phenotypes, or interact with genetic factors [5,31]. Finally, 99 other differences between studies such as subject characteristics (age, 100 gender, health, ethnicity, sample size) and behavioral measures (verbal 101 Q14 or physical aggression, suicide, antisocial behavior, etc.) may also have 102 contributed to the inconsistencies in the literature on gene-aggression 103 association. 104 The current study adopted a system-level approach to examine 105 the role of the serotoninergic system and environmental factors as 106 well as their interaction in aggressive behavior in a relatively ho107 mogenous sample (in terms of age, health status, and ethnicity). To 108 do this, we enrolled a sample of young healthy Han Chinese college 109 students, genotyped 129 polymorphic loci within the serotonin 110 system, and assessed relevant environmental factors and aggressive 111 behavior. Because gender has been found to be a potential confound112 ing factor [32], we included it as a covariate.

D

65 66

E

2

Mean (SD)

Aggression Questionnaire (AQ) Parental warmth Stressful life events

Cronbach's alpha

53.00 (7.49) 9.72 (5.08)

0.827 –

146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162

165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173

Correlations

t1:3

AQ

t1:4

Parental warmth

68.57 (15.75) 0.855 ⁎

144 145

−0.17⁎⁎ 0.29⁎⁎ −0.17⁎⁎

⁎ p b 0.05. ⁎⁎ p b 0.01.

Please cite this article as: Chen C, et al, Genetic variations in the serotoninergic system and environmental factors contribute to aggressive behavior in Chinese adolescents, Physiol Behav (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.09.005

t1:5 t1:6 Q3 t1:7 t1:8 t1:9 Q4 t1:10

C. Chen et al. / Physiology & Behavior xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190

208 209 210 211 Q16 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 Q17 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 Q18 232 233 234 235 Q19 236

The goal of the current study was to understand the relation between individual differences in aggressive behavior, environmental factors, and genetic variations in the 5-HT system in healthy subjects. Moving beyond the single-gene or a small number of haplotype approaches used in typical molecular behavior genetics research, this study used the system-level approach [29] to examine the overall contributions of the serotoninergic system (characterized by the major genes and their associated loci) and environmental factors on aggressive behavior. Briefly, the analysis includes three steps: First, ANOVA was used to screen polymorphic loci that showed nominal significance (p b 0.05) on aggressive behavior; second, these loci, two environmental factors (stressful life events and parental warmth), gender, and their interactions were then entered into a regression model to estimate their overall contribution to aggressive behavior; and lastly the regression model was verified by permutation. In this study, we built three kinds of regression models. In Model 1, we included gender, environmental factors, and the loci with significant main effects based on the ANOVA results and used the forward stepwise method to build the model. To run multiple regression analyses, all SNPs were coded in a linear way, i.e., 1 = major allele homozygotes, 2 = heterozygotes, and 3 = minor allele homozygotes. SNPs on the X chromosome were coded as 1 = major allele and 3 = minor allele for males because they have only one copy of the X chromosome; to be consistent, 1 = major allele homozygotes and 3 = minor allele carriers (both heterozygotes and minor allele homozygotes) for females. The MAOA VNTR on the X chromosome was coded as 1 = 3 repeat and 3 = 4 repeat for males; 1 = 3 repeat homozygotes and 3 = 4 repeat carriers for females. In Model 2, all two-way interactions of these loci in Model 1 were added using a forward stepwise method. In Model 3, two-way interactions of these loci and environmental factors were added to Model 2 using a forward stepwise method. Permutations were done by shuffling aggressive behavior (along with gender and environmental factors) across subjects 1000 times, and recalculating probabilities. The probability of getting a larger R2 in shuffled data than in real data was defined as the p value of the model.

240 241

P

D

E

T

C

206 207

E

205

R

203 204

R

201 202

N C O

199 200

U

197 198

2.3.3. Gene data preprocessing Two subjects with more than 10% null genotypes were excluded. In addition to automatic calling of genotypes, the Illumina genotyping platform supplies a quantitative quality measure known as the GenCall score. It measures how close a genotype is to the center of the cluster of other samples assigned to the same genotypes, compared with the centers of the clusters of the other genotypes. This measure ranges from 0 to 1, with a higher score indicating a more reliable result. The conventional cutoff point is .25 [42]. Of the 60 228 genotypes (126 SNPs by 478 subjects) in the current study, 120 genotypes (0.2%) were excluded because their GenCall scores were lower than .25. Additional data cleaning included the treatment of low-frequency alleles. For SNPs with either heterozygote or minor allele homozygotes found in fewer than 10 (about 2%) participants, these two genotype groups were combined. If the combined group still had fewer than 10 participants, the SNP(s) were excluded in further analysis. Purported SNPs that turned out to be monomorphic were also deleted. The Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) index was calculated using the Chi square test and setting df to 1. Since males have only one X chromosome, only females were included in HWE calculation for SNPs located on X chromosome. Five of the SNPs showed significant HW disequilibrium (p b 0.01). However, we still kept them (although none of them found to be associated with aggression) because we determined that they did not seem to have resulted from genotyping error but rather reflected the characters of our college student sample [43]. The inclusion of both tSNPs and additional SNPs in regions detected in selection screens [44,45] resulted in high LD among a number of SNPs. Thirty SNPs included in initial analysis were excluded from multiple regression analysis because of their high LD with other adjacent SNPs (R2 N 0.8, calculated with Plink [46]), yielding a final list of 99 polymorphisms for the main data analyses. It is worth mentioning that the “redundant” SNPs showed the same or almost the same results as the linked SNPs, confirming the association. Online Supplementary Table S1 shows the details of all 129 polymorphic loci (127 SNPs and 2 VNTRs) included in our study: location (rs number, chromosome, position), gene, serotonin subsystem, allele polymorphism and frequency, Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium, linkage disequilibrium, and deleted SNPs. Finally, the genetic relatedness of subjects was checked following the Anderson et al. [47] protocol using Plink. We used all 240 unrelated autosome SNPs (R2 b 0.8) available in the larger project of these subjects and set the threshold at 0.95 (personal communication with Drs. Anderson

239

R O

2.3.2. Genotyping techniques 191 The SNPs were genotyped using the standard Illumina Golden Gate 192 Genotyping protocol (see Illumina Golden-Gate Assay Protocol for de193 tails, http://www.southgene.com.cn; Shanghai South Gene Technology 194 Q15 Co., Ltd, Shanghai, China). In addition, 5HTTLPR and MAOA VNTR were 195 ascertained by standard PCR procedures [39–41]. 196

2.4. Statistical analysis

F

176 177

and Zondervan). We found no pair of subjects showing high relatedness 237 (all PI_HAT smaller than or equal to 0.5). 238

subsystem includes (1) SLC6A4, an integral membrane-spanning protein that pumps the neurotransmitter serotonin from synaptic spaces into presynaptic neurons and (2) VMAT, a transport protein integrated into the membrane of intracellular vesicles of presynaptic neurons, which acts to transport monoamines into the synaptic vesicles. We included SLC6A4 (7 SNPs plus 5HTTLPR), VMAT1 (SLC18A1, 9 SNPs), and VMAT2 (SLC18A2, 5 SNPs). For the receptor subsystem, we included all 17 known genes (with the respective number of SNPs in parentheses): HTR1A (2), HTR1B (2), HTR1D (13), HTR1F (5), HTR2A (21), HTR2B (6), HTR2C (3), HTR3A (1), HTR3B (2), HTR3C (3), HTR3D (4), HTR3E (2), HTR4 (10), HTR5A (4), HTR5B (2), HTR6 (5), and HTR7 (7). These genes represent all major genes involved in these four 5-HT subsystems in humans, and SNPs were selected based on capturing most known polymorphisms through LD (linkage disequilibrium) [38]. Details about these genes and the selected loci can be found in Supporting information Table S1.

O

174 175

3

242 243 Q20 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 Q21 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275

3. Results

276

Table 1 shows the mean total score, standard deviations, reliability estimates, and inter-correlations among the self-report measures. Aggressive behavior was negatively correlated with parental warmth, and was positively correlated with the number of stressful life events. Parental warmth was negatively correlated with the number of stressful life events. Of the 99 polymorphisms that survived LD testing, 12 showed main effects with p b 0.05. Individuals who were major allele homozygotes for rs10917509 (HTR6) or rs1328684 (HTR2A), or rs909525 (MAOA), or minor allele homozygotes for rs6658108 (HTR6) or rs9534501 (HTR2A) or rs2770296 (HTR2A) reported higher aggressive behaviors, whereas individuals who were major allele homozygotes for rs1487275 (TPH2) or rs8076005 (SLC6A4), heterozygotes for rs732050 (HTR5A) or rs6651806 (MAOB) or rs2192371 (HTR2C) or rs6644065 (HTR2C) reported fewer aggressive behaviors (Table 2, and online Supplementary Table S2 for detailed results for all 129 loci). These 12 SNPs and environmental factors plus gender were used in a regression analysis to build models. Table 3 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis. In step one, gender and both of the environmental factors were entered into the regression equation, accounting for 11% (10.7% adjusted) of the variance in aggressive behavior. Forward stepwise regression was then used and five of the 12 SNPs

277

Please cite this article as: Chen C, et al, Genetic variations in the serotoninergic system and environmental factors contribute to aggressive behavior in Chinese adolescents, Physiol Behav (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.09.005

278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298

4 t2:1 t2:2 t2:3

C. Chen et al. / Physiology & Behavior xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

Table 2 Means and standard deviations of the AQ score, and main effects and post hoc comparisons of SNPs that showed significant main effects and used in subsequent multiple regression analysis. SNP

Subsystem

Gene

Maj

Mean

SD

N

Het

Mean

SD

N

Min

Mean

SD

N

F

p

Post hoc (p b 0.05)

t2:5 t2:6 t2:7 t2:8 t2:9 t2:10 t2:11 t2:12 t2:13 t2:14 t2:15 t2:16

rs1487275 rs909525 rs6651806 rs8076005 rs10917509 rs6658108 rs732050 rs9534501 rs2770296 rs1328684 rs2192371 rs6644065

Synthesis Degradation Degradation Transport Receptor Receptor Receptor Receptor Receptor Receptor Receptor Receptor

TPH2 MAOA MAOB SLC6A4 HTR6 HTR6 HTR5A HTR2A HTR2A HTR2A HTR2C HTR2C

AA GG AA AA AA GG AA GG AA AA AA AA

66.72 70.23 69.36 67.48 69.66 67.54 70.31 68.13 68.61 69.26 70.37 69.42

15.00 15.95 16.03 15.11 16.42 15.35 16.23 15.50 15.95 16.07 17.50 16.27

191 217 381 355 309 280 302 303 265 406 242 373

AC AG AC AG AG AG AG AG AG AG AG AG

68.95 67.16 65.48 71.41 66.59 69.03 65.32 68.20 67.27 64.71 66.72 65.57

15.42 15.50 14.26 16.37 14.28 15.68 14.04 14.53 13.84 13.27 13.52 13.39

223 260 97 111 169 168 157 153 181 72 236 105

CC AA CC GG GG AA GG AA GG GG GG GG

72.77

18.24

64

74.58

23.98

12

75.60 67.68 77.27 75.63

18.28 17.76 23.86 21.88

30 19 22 32

3.69 4.53 4.71 3.57 4.18 3.70 5.31 3.56 3.88 5.14 6.48 4.92

0.03 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.04 0.03 0.00 0.03 0.02 0.02 0.01 0.03

AAbCC GGNAG AANAC AAbAG AANAG GG, AGbAA AANAG GG, AGbAA AA, AGbGG AANAG AANAG AANAG

O

299 300

E

D

P

R O

Only by summing up their overall effects can we understand the genetic basis of a complex trait such as aggressive behavior. TPH2 is expressed in the brain of humans and is responsible for the central nervous system effects of 5-HT. Previous studies found that the C1473G polymorphism in the TPH2 gene was involved in the determination of TPH activity and was linked to inter-male aggression in inbred mouse strains [48,49]. The current study identified that the A allele of the rs1487275 polymorphism of the TPH2 gene was associated with lower aggression. This result is consistent with previous results that the A allele was associated with higher openness and lower agreeableness measured by the NEO-Five Factor Inventory [50] and less social phobia [51], suggesting that people carrying the A allele may be better at handling interpersonal relationship and are thus less aggressive. Previous research has shown a consistent association between the level of MAOA and aggression [10,12,13,52]. In terms of the MAOA gene, the most often studied polymorphism involves variable-number tandem repeats (VNTR) in the upstream region of the MAOA gene. Its association with aggressive behavior, however, has not always been consistent. Among the replicated results was the MAOA VNTR by childhood abuse interaction on adult aggression [13]. We did not find a significant association between this VNTR polymorphism and aggressive behavior, but we did find an effect of MAOA rs909525, which was previously reported to be associated with higher levels of anger expressed outwards in male suicidal patients and associated with suicidality in males [53], openness in NEO-FFI, and problem gambling [50]. Our results and those of Antypa et al.'s both suggest that the G allele carriers are more aggressive. We also found a main effect of MAOB rs6651806, which was associated with negative emotionality, stress reaction, alienation, and aggression in an earlier study [54]. The A allele was associated with higher aggression scores in both studies. Consistent with previous studies, SLC6A4 was associated with aggressive behavior. The polymorphism in SNP rs8076005 that showed

4. Discussion

R

319

E

C

T

entered the model, accounting for 17% (15% adjusted) of the variance in aggressive behavior, F (8, 468) = 11.63, p = 3.89 ∗ 10−15. In step two, 301 we added in two-way interaction terms for these SNPs using the for302 ward stepwise procedure. There were 10 potential SNP–SNP interac303 tions and 1 of them entered the model. In Model 2, the R2 increased to 304 0.18 and adjusted R2 to 0.16, F (9, 468) = 11.03, p = 1.22 ∗ 10−15. In 305 Model 3, the two-way interaction terms of SNPs and environmental fac306 tors were added using the forward stepwise procedure. There were 10 307 potential SNP–environment interactions and 1 of them entered the 308 model. The final model accounted for 19% (17% adjusted) of the vari309 ance in aggressive behavior, F (10, 467) = 10.70, p = 2.22 ∗ 10−16. 310 To validate these results, Monte Carlo permutation was conducted. 311 Fig. 1 shows the permutation results of R2. In each subplot, the X axis 312 represents R2, and the Y axis represents the number of occurrences of 313 a given R2 in 1000 permutations. The curve represents the distribution 314 of R2 based on the permutated data, whereas the vertical line indicates 315 actual R2 obtained in this study. Based on these permutations, the prob316 ability of obtaining the R2 or adjusted R2 found in Model 1 was 0.073 and 317 0.088, respectively. The corresponding probabilities for Model 2 were 318 Q22 0.058 and 0.065 and for Model 3 were 0.045 and 0.053.

F

t2:4

320 321

t3:1 t3:2 t3:3

U

N

C

O

R

Based on the system-level analysis of 5-HT neurotransmitter genes, we identified 12 SNPs of eight 5-HT-related genes (TPH2, MAOA, 322 MAOB, SLC6A4, HTR2A, HTR2C, HTR5A, and HTR6) showing nominal 323 effects on aggressive behavior. Five of these SNPs made significant unique 324 contributions to aggressive behavior. Together with environmental 325 Q23 factors (parental warmth and acceptance and stressful life events), they 326 accounted for up to 19% of individual difference in aggressive behavior, 327 confirming that aggressive behavior was likely to be influenced to some 328 extent by the serotoninergic system as well as environmental factors.

Table 3 Three regression models for AQ with both genetic data and environmental variables. Regressor

Gene 1

Gene 2

t3:4 t3:5 t3:6 t3:7 t3:8 t3:9 t3:10 t3:11 t3:12 t3:13 Q5 t3:14 t3:15

Gender Parental warmth Stress rs6658108 rs732050 rs1487275 rs8076005 rs2192371 rs732050–rs2192371 rs2192371–stress

HTR6 HTR5A TPH2 SLC6A4 HTR2C HTR5A HTR2C

HTR2C

Model 1

Model 2

Model 3

B

T

p

B

T

p

B

T

p

−3.90 −0.24 0.78 3.03 −2.75 2.22 3.38 −1.69

−2.88 −2.67 5.83 2.77 −2.32 2.25 2.54 −2.06

0.00 0.01 0.00 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.01 0.04

−3.94 −0.22 0.77 3.04 −2.72 2.29 3.27 −1.61 3.33

−2.93 −2.49 5.76 2.79 −2.32 2.34 2.48 −1.97 2.28

0.00 0.01 0.00 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.01 0.05 0.02

−3.92 −0.21 0.81 3.31 −2.55 2.17 3.20 2.22 3.11 −0.39

−2.94 −2.28 6.06 3.05 −2.18 2.22 2.43 1.30 2.14 −2.56

0.00 0.02 0.00 0.00 0.03 0.03 0.02 0.19 0.03 0.01

Note: ‘Gene 1’ and ‘Gene 2’ are the corresponding genes for each SNP; ‘B’ is the regression coefficient, ‘T’ and ‘p’ are t-test results.

Please cite this article as: Chen C, et al, Genetic variations in the serotoninergic system and environmental factors contribute to aggressive behavior in Chinese adolescents, Physiol Behav (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.09.005

329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360

C. Chen et al. / Physiology & Behavior xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

5

N C O

R

R

E

C

T

E

D

P

R O

O

F

receptors. However, in the present study, we did not find any significant associations between polymorphic variations in the 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptor genes and aggressive behavior in a Chinese population. These null results were in accordance with Takahashi et al.'s review [22] that reported no effects of the 5-HT1A gene and inconsistent effects of the 5-HT1B gene on aggression [also see 23–25]. We should hasten to add that this lack of (consistent) results should not automatically rule out a potential role of these genes in aggression because our SNPs (two per gene in this case) as well as those used in previous studies might have missed the causal loci. In contrast to the above two genes, our analysis revealed that polymorphisms in HTR2A, HTR2C, HTR5A and HTR6 were associated with aggressive behavior in Chinese subjects. Several studies have found significant associations between 5-HT2A (T102C/ rs6313, A1438G/rs6311 and His452Tyr/rs6314) and extraversion, aggressive impulsive trait, impaired behavior control, or adolescentonset antisocial behavior in humans [57–61]. The significant polymorphism rs1328684 in the current study was reported to be associated with extraversion scores [50] as well as treatment response in obsessive–compulsive disorder patients to serotonin reuptake inhibitors [62] and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder [63]. In addition, we found that the rs2770296 A allele was associated with less aggression, whereas previous studies showed that this allele was protective against bipolar disorder [64] and associated with novelty seeking [65]. There is little previous work on the remaining 5-HT receptors with respect to aggressive behavior, and much work remains in order to clarify the specific roles of the 5-HT2C, 5-HT5A and 5-HT6 receptors in aggressive behavior found in our study. It should be noted that we found significant HTR5A-by-HTR2C and HTR2C-by-stressful life events interactions, suggesting complex relations within receptor-related genes. Epistasis and epigenetic effects have been frequently reported. For example, Reuter reported an interaction between COMT and DRD2 on personality and suggested that a balance between degradation and receptor affinity may result in positive emotionality [66]. Here the HTR5A-by-HTR2C interaction may suggest a competition mechanism between receptors. Also stressful life events have been reported to interact with MAOA [67], COMT [68], and BDNF [69] on aggressive behavior, suggesting that genes may predispose individuals to aggressive behavior but environmental triggers are also needed for individuals to act aggressively. Further research on an HTR2C–environment interaction is needed. Several limitations of the current study need to be mentioned. First, we focused only on healthy Han Chinese college students, so these results may or may not be generalized to other populations (e.g., clinical samples, other ethnic groups). Second, we selected 129 polymorphic loci related to the 5-HT system according to our previous study and HapMap information to cover variations of the major serotoninrelated genes. A more extensive coverage may reveal more variants with significant effects. In addition, other neuronal systems such as the dopamine system may also contribute to aggressive behavior [70–72]. Such a system should be examined separately or in conjunction with the 5-HT system. Last, we used only two environmental factors in our model. A careful selection of other environmental factors or other measures of parenting and social environments would help refine our model. Future research should also incorporate other examples of gene–environment interactions. In conclusion, the current study used a system-level approach to study gene-behavior associations that considered the polygenic nature of behavior and examined gene–gene interactions and incorporated both genetic and environmental factors. Our results suggest that many genes within the serotonin system may contribute to aggression, each with a small effect and potentially interacting with each other as well as environmental factors. By summing up their effects, one fifth of the variance of aggression can be explained. These results also suggest that the effect of genes within serotonin system can be cumulative and the system-level approach may be a promising way to explore the genetic basis of human aggressive behavior.

U

Fig. 1. Permutation results for the three models: Model 1 (first row), Model 2 (second row) and Model 3 (third row). The dashed line represents empirical distribution of R2 obtained from the randomized data, and the solid vertical line represents R2 obtained from the actual data.

361 362

a significant effect in the current study is located in the intron region of SLC6A4. A recent study found that this polymorphism was significantly 363 associated with depression measured by the Beck Depression Inventory 364 and plasma interleukin-6 level [55]. In that study, the G allele 365 carriers tended to be more depressed, and in our study they were 366 more aggressive. The G allele was also found to be associated with 367 ADHD in females [56]. 368 Q24 As summarized in the Introduction section, with regard to 5-HT 369 receptors, studies mainly concentrated on the 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B

Please cite this article as: Chen C, et al, Genetic variations in the serotoninergic system and environmental factors contribute to aggressive behavior in Chinese adolescents, Physiol Behav (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.09.005

370 371 372 373 374 375 Q25 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435

438

Acknowledgments

439 Q26

444

This study was supported by the Research Fund for the Doctoral Program of Higher Education (20110003120001), the 111 Project (B07008) of the Ministry of Education of China, the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (2012LYB05), and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31100807 and 31221003). We thank all graduate research assistants who helped with data collection.

445

References

446 Q27 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511

[1] Krug EG, Dahlberg LL, Mercy JA, Zwi AB, Loz ano R. World report on violence and health. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2002. [2] Ferguson CJ. Genetic contributions to antisocial personality and behavior: a meta-analytic review from an evolutionary perspective. J Soc Psychol 2010;150: 160–80. [3] Miles DR, Carey G. Genetic and environmental architecture on human aggression. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;72:207–17. [4] Craig IW, Halton KE. Genetics of human aggressive behaviour. Hum Genet 2009;126: 101–13. [5] Mendes DD, de Jesus Mari J, Singer M, Barros GM, Mello AF. Study review of biological, social and environmental factors associated with aggressive behavior. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 2009;31:S77–85. [6] Pavlov KA, Chistiakov DA, Chekhonin VP. Genetic determinants of aggression and impulsivity in humans. J Appl Genet 2012;53:61–82. [7] Nelson RJ, Chiavegatto S. Molecular basis of aggression. Trends Neurosci 2001;24: 713–9. [8] Lesch KP, Merschdorf U. Impulsivity, aggression, and serotonin: a molecular psychobiological perspective. Behav Sci Law 2000;18:581–604. [9] Popova NK. From genes to aggressive behavior: the role of serotonergic system. Bioessays 2006;28:495–503. [10] Florvall L, Ask AL, Ogren SO, Ross SB. Selective monoamine oxidase inhibitors. 1. Compounds related to 4-aminophenethylamine. J Med Chem 1978;21:56–63. [11] Hurd PL, Vaillancourt KL, Dinsdale NL. Aggression, digit ratio and variation in androgen receptor and monoamine oxidase A genes in men. Behav Genet 2011; 41:543–56. [12] Hessl D, Tassone F, Cordeiro L, Koldewyn K, McCormick C, Green C, et al. Brief report: Aggression and stereotypic behavior in males with fragile X syndrome—moderating secondary genes in a “single gene” disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 2008;38:184–9. [13] Buckholtz JW, Meyer-Lindenberg A. MAOA and the neurogenetic architecture of human aggression. Trends Neurosci 2008;31:120–9. [14] Holmes A, Murphy DL, Crawley JN. Reduced aggression in mice lacking the serotonin transporter. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;161:160–7. [15] Han DH, Park DB, Na C, Kee BS, Lee YS. Association of aggressive behavior in Korean male schizophrenic patients with polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter promoter and catecholamine-O-methyltransferase genes. Psychiatry Res 2004;129: 29–37. [16] Beitchman J, Baldassarra L, Mik H, De Luca V, King N, Bender D, et al. Serotonin transporter polymorphisms and persistent, pervasive childhood aggression. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:1103–5. [17] Lesch K-P, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 1996;274:1527–31. [18] Liao D-L, Hong C-J, Shih H-L, Tsai S-J. Possible association between serotonin transporter promoter region polymorphism and extremely violent crime in Chinese males. Neuropsychobiology 2004;50:284–7. [19] Parsey RV, Oquendo MA, Simpson NR, Ogden RT, Van Heertum R, Arango V, et al. Effects of sex, age, and aggressive traits in man on brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptor binding potential measured by PET using [C-11] WAY-100635. Brain Res 2002;954: 173–82. [20] Popova NK, Naumenko VS, Plyusnina IZ, Kulikov AV. Reduction in 5‐HT1A receptor density, 5‐HT1A mRNA expression, and functional correlates for 5‐HT1A receptors in genetically defined aggressive rats. J Neurosci Res 2005;80:286–92. [21] Soyka M, Preuss UW, Koller G, Zill P, Bondy B. Association of 5-HT1B receptor gene and antisocial behavior in alcoholism. J Neural Transm 2004;111:101–9. [22] Takahashi A, Quadros IM, de Almeida RM, Miczek KA. Brain serotonin receptors and transporters: initiation vs. termination of escalated aggression. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011;213:183–212. [23] Nishiguchi N, Shirakawa O, Ono H, Nishimura A, Nushida H, Ueno Y, et al. Brief research communication — no evidence of an association between 5HT1B receptor gene polymorphism and suicide victims in a Japanese population. Am J Med Genet 2001;105:343–5. [24] Rujescu D, Giegling I, Sato T, Moller HJ. Lack of association between serotonin 5-HT1B receptor gene polymorphism and suicidal behavior. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2003;116B:69–71. [25] Stefulj J, Buttner A, Skavic J, Zill P, Balija M, Eisenmenger W, et al. Serotonin 1B (5HT1B) receptor polymorphism (G861C) in suicide victims: association studies in

[28]

440 441 442 443

[29] [30]

[31] [32]

[33]

[34]

[35] [36]

[37]

[38] [39]

E

[40]

[41]

T

C

E

R

R

O

C

N

U

F

[27]

O

[26]

None.

German and Slavic population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2004; 127B:48–50. Fisher RAXV. The correlation between relatives on the supposition of Mendelian inheritance. Trans R Soc Edinb 1919;52:399–433. Yang J, Benyamin B, McEvoy BP, Gordon S, Henders AK, Nyholt DR, et al. Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height. Nat Genet 2010;42:565–9. Chen C, Chen C, Moyzis R, Stern H, He Q, Li H, et al. Contributions of dopaminerelated genes and environmental factors to highly sensitive personality: a multistep neuronal system-level approach. PLoS One 2011;6:e21636. Chen C, Chen C, Moyzis R, He Q, Li H, Li J, et al. Genetic variations in the dopaminergic system and alcohol use: a system‐level analysis. Addict Biol 2011;17:479–89. Bou-Flores C, Hilaire G. 5-Hydroxytryptamine(2A) and 5-hydroxytryptamine(1B) receptors are differently affected by the monoamine oxidase A-deficiency in the Tg8 transgenic mouse. Neurosci Lett 2000;296:141–4. Liu J. Early health risk factors for violence: conceptualization, evidence, and implications. Aggress Violent Behav 2011;16:63–73. Wakschlag LS, Kistner EO, Pine DS, Biesecker G, Pickett KE, Skol A, et al. Interaction of prenatal exposure to cigarettes and MAOA genotype in pathways to youth antisocial behavior. Mol Psychiatry 2009;15:928–37. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption‐II. Addiction 1993;88:791–804. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerstrom KO. The Fagerström test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict 1991;86:1119–27. Buss AH, Perry M. The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol 1992;63:452. Greenberger E, Chen C, Tally SR, Dong Q. Family, peer, and individual correlates of depressive symptomatology among US and Chinese adolescents. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000;68:209. Wills TA, Vaccaro D, McNamara G. The role of life events, family support, and competence in adolescent substance use: a test of vulnerability and protective factors. Am J Community Psychol 1992;20:349–74. The International Human Genome Sequencing Consortium. Finishing the euchromatic sequence of the human genome. Nature 2004;431:931–45. Sabol SZ, Hu S, Hamer D. A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Hum Genet 1998;103:273–9. Qian Q, Wang Y, Zhou R, Li J, Wang B, Glatt S, et al. Family-based and case–control association studies of catechol-O-methyltransferase in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder suggest genetic sexual dimorphism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2003;118B:103–9. Collier DA, Stober G, Li T, Heils A, Catalano M, Di Bella D, et al. A novel functional polymorphism within the promoter of the serotonin transporter gene: possible role in susceptibility to affective disorders. Mol Psychiatry 1996;1:453–60. Guan L, Wang B, Chen Y, Yang L, Li J, Qian Q, et al. A high-density single-nucleotide polymorphism screen of 23 candidate genes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: suggesting multiple susceptibility genes among Chinese Han population. Mol Psychiatry 2009;14:546–54. Chen C, Chen C, Moyzis R, He Q, Lei X, Li J, et al. Genotypes over-represented among college students are linked to better cognitive abilities and socioemotional adjustment. Culture Brain 2013;1:47–63. Wang ET, Kodama G, Baldi P, Moyzis RK. Global landscape of recent inferred Darwinian selection for Homo sapiens. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103: 135–40. Hawks J, Wang ET, Cochran GM, Harpending HC, Moyzis RK. Recent acceleration of human adaptive evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007;104:20753–8. Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MA, Bender D, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet 2007;81:559–75. Anderson CA, Pettersson FH, Clarke GM, Cardon LR, Morris AP, Zondervan KT. Data quality control in genetic case–control association studies. Nat Protoc 2010;5: 1564–73. Kulikov A, Osipova D, Naumenko V, Popova N. Association between Tph2 gene polymorphism, brain tryptophan hydroxylase activity and aggressiveness in mouse strains. Genes Brain Behav 2005;4:482–5. Osipova DV, Kulikov AV, Popova NK. C1473G polymorphism in mouse tph2 gene is linked to tryptophan hydroxylase‐2 activity in the brain, intermale aggression, and depressive‐like behavior in the forced swim test. J Neurosci Res 2009;87: 1168–74. Tong R. Association studies of personality traits, problem gambling, and serotonergic gene polymorphisms. University of Toronto; 2011. Serretti A, Liappas I, Mandelli L, Albani D, Forloni G, Malitas P, et al. TPH2 gene variants and anxiety during alcohol detoxification outcome. Psychiatry Res 2009;167: 106–14. Brunner HG, Nelen M, Breakefield X, Ropers H, Van Oost B. Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science 1993;262:578. Antypa N, Giegling I, Calati R, Schneider B, Hartmann AM, Friedl M, et al. MAOA and MAOB polymorphisms and anger-related traits in suicidal participants and controls. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2013;263:393–403. Dlugos AM, Palmer AA, de Wit H. Negative emotionality: monoamine oxidase B gene variants modulate personality traits in healthy humans. J Neural Transm 2009;116: 1323–34. Su S, Zhao J, Bremner JD, Miller AH, Tang W, Bouzyk M, et al. Serotonin transporter gene, depressive symptoms, and interleukin-6. Circ Cardiovasc Genet 2009;2: 614–20.

R O

437

Conflict of interest

P

436

C. Chen et al. / Physiology & Behavior xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

D

6

[42]

[43]

[44]

[45] [46]

[47]

[48]

[49]

[50] [51]

[52]

[53]

[54]

[55]

Please cite this article as: Chen C, et al, Genetic variations in the serotoninergic system and environmental factors contribute to aggressive behavior in Chinese adolescents, Physiol Behav (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.09.005

512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597

C. Chen et al. / Physiology & Behavior xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

[65] Heck A, Lieb R, Ellgas A, Pfister H, Lucae S, Roeske D, et al. Investigation of 17 candidate genes for personality traits confirms effects of the HTR2A gene on novelty seeking. Genes Brain Behav 2009;8:464–72. [66] Reuter M, Schmitz A, Corr P, Hennig J. Molecular genetics support Gray's personality theory: the interaction of COMT and DRD2 polymorphisms predicts the behavioural approach system. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2006;9:155–66. [67] Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW, et al. Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science 2002;297:851–4. [68] Wagner S, Baskaya Ö, Anicker N, Dahmen N, Lieb K, Tadić A. The catechol o‐methyltransferase (COMT) val158met polymorphism modulates the association of serious life events (SLE) and impulsive aggression in female patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD). Acta Psychiatr Scand 2010;122:110–7. [69] Wagner S, Baskaya Ö, Dahmen N, Lieb K, Tadić A. Modulatory role of the brain‐ derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism on the effects of serious life events on impulsive aggression in borderline personality disorder. Genes Brain Behav 2010;9:97–102. [70] Guo G, Roettger ME, Shih JC. Contributions of the DAT1 and DRD2 genes to serious and violent delinquency among adolescents and young adults. Hum Genet 2007; 121:125–36. [71] Eisenberg D, MacKillop J, Modi M, Beauchemin J, Dang D, Lisman SA, et al. Examining impulsivity as an endophenotype using a behavioral approach: a DRD2 TaqI A and DRD4 48-bp VNTR association study. Behav Brain Funct 2007;3. [72] Beaver KM, Wright JP, DeLisi M, Walsh A, Vaughn MG, Boisvert D, et al. A gene × gene interaction between DRD2 and DRD4 is associated with conduct disorder and antisocial behavior in males. Behav Brain Funct 2007;3:1186.

N C O

R

R

E

C

T

E

D

P

R O

O

F

[56] Landaas E, Johansson S, Jacobsen K, Ribases M, Bosch R, Sánchez‐Mora, C, et al. An international multicenter association study of the serotonin transporter gene in persistent ADHD. Genes Brain Behav 2010;9:449–58. [57] Ni X, Bismil R, Chan K, Sicard T, Bulgin N, McMain S, et al. Serotonin 2A receptor gene is associated with personality traits, but not to disorder, in patients with borderline personality disorder. Neurosci Lett 2006;408:214–9. [58] Assal F, Alarcón M, Solomon EC, Masterman D, Geschwind DH, Cummings JL. Association of the serotonin transporter and receptor gene polymorphisms in neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 2004;61:1249. [59] Bjork JM, Moeller FG, Dougherty DM, Swann AC, Machado MA, Hanis CL. Serotonin 2a receptor T102C polymorphism and impaired impulse control. Am J Med Genet 2002;114:336–9. [60] Burt SA, Mikolajewski AJ. Preliminary evidence that specific candidate genes are associated with adolescent‐onset antisocial behavior. Aggress Behav 2008;34:437–45. [61] Nomura M, Kusumi I, Kaneko M, Masui T, Daiguji M, Ueno T, et al. Involvement of a polymorphism in the 5-HT2A receptor gene in impulsive behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;187:30–5. [62] Brandl EJ, Müller DJ, Richter MA. Pharmacogenetics of obsessive–compulsive disorders. Pharmacogenomics 2012;13:71–81. [63] Ribases M, Ramos-Quiroga J, Hervas A, Bosch R, Bielsa A, Gastaminza X, et al. Exploration of 19 serotoninergic candidate genes in adults and children with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder identifies association for 5HT2A, DDC and MAOB. Mol Psychiatry 2007;14:71–85. [64] McAuley EZ, Fullerton JM, Blair IP, Donald JA, Mitchell PB, Schofield PR. Association between the serotonin 2A receptor gene and bipolar affective disorder in an Australian cohort. Psychiatr Genet 2009;19:244–52.

U

598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 649

7

Please cite this article as: Chen C, et al, Genetic variations in the serotoninergic system and environmental factors contribute to aggressive behavior in Chinese adolescents, Physiol Behav (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.09.005

624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 643 644 645 646 647 648

Genetic variations in the serotoninergic system and environmental factors contribute to aggressive behavior in Chinese adolescents.

Aggressive behavior is a major public health problem worldwide and has been associated with many gene variants, especially those related to the seroto...
522KB Sizes 0 Downloads 5 Views