Perception, 1977, volume 6, pages 601 -602

Illusory line linking solid rods

Colin Ware, John M Kennedy Division of Life Sciences, Scarborough College, University of Toronto, West Hill, Ontario M1C 1A4, Canada Received 1 February 1977

Abstract. A solid object—a frame enclosing rods—can be seen as having an illusory 'line' joining the tips of the rods. Illusory contours are apparent lines and divisions cutting across regions of the perceived display which are actually physically homogeneous (Osgood 1951). Figure 1 is a photograph of a three-dimensional object, which allows the viewer to perceive an illusory contour. Whether viewed directly, or via a photograph, the three-dimensional object can be seen as having a peculiar 'line' linking the tips of the rods. This 'line' is visible in static inspection of the object, in monocular and binocular inspection, and in inspection during head motion, motion of the object, and motion of the background. Fixation can be on the background (near or far) or the eyes can track the object, and the 'line' will still be seen; although, as is usual with illusory contours, some subjects report that sections of the 'line' (between adjacent rods) disappear when they are directly inspected, notably in the static conditions. Previous experimenters have used two-dimensional displays to create illusory contours and perceived depth. It has been noted that in these displays it could be

Figure 1. Photograph of a three-dimensional object, approximately 20 cm x 12 cm x 2 cm. Inspection of the object results in an impression of an 'illusory contour' joining the ends of the rods. Viewed directly, rather than in a photograph, the compelling impression is of a 'line' linking coplanar rods, though stratification in depth is readily possible given a picture of the object, as here.

602

C Ware, J M Kennedy

that the contour is a result of depth cues forcing the viewer to perceive nonexistent stratification in depth (Kanizsa 1976; Gregory 1972). The depth-cue theory has also been argued to hold for some binocular displays that result in illusory contours (Coren 1972). The object shown in figure 1 has been inspected by many subjects, some of whom have viewed it on many occasions and for prolonged periods (with considerable interest!). The 'line' has been noted by the viewers to be present without any perceived stratification in depth; that is, all the rods are seen as in the same depth, and as linked by the 'line'. Presumably when looking at two-dimensional graphic displays, the viewer is free to exercise pictorial depth-perception skills and to create figure-ground reversals, etc. A three-dimensional display provides binocular cues, head motion, object motion, and background motion as information for depth. The viewer of such a display is much more constrained and thus it can be shown that depth stratification is not necessary for perception of subjective contour. We have made other solid objects (and also shapes on transparent surfaces) which induce subjective figures. We have used both opaque and semitransparent materials in their construction. It is of interest to note that the subjective figure can be seen as 'space filling' (i.e., voluminous or 'solid') not just as linear or planar. Whether linear, planar, or 'space-filling', the subjective figures can be seen as unambiguously located at the same distance as the inducing strips or rods, not as overlapping the inducing materials or as part of the background. Evidently, the stratification-in-depth hypothesis is unsatisfactory as a general explanation of subjective contours. Hence, alternative explanations are required, for example the recent ones which invoke brightness contrast effects tied to appropriate stimulus groupings (Kanizsa 1976), notably groupings of line and rod endings (Frisby and Clatworthy 1975; Kennedy and Lee 1976). Of course, the object of figure 1 leaves open the possibility that there are particular conditions where the stratification-in-depth hypothesis is the most satisfactory explanation. Just such a condition may be the one in which the form information is binocular disparity (rather than the monocular brightness differences that are the substrate for contrast effects). The key point that our object makes is that illusory contours can be produced without prior stratification being the causal agent. References Coren S, 1972 "Subjective contours and apparent depth" Psychological Review 79 339-367 Frisby J P, Clatworthy J L, 1975 "Illusory contours: curious cases of simultaneous brightness contrast" Perception 4 349-347 Gregory R L, 1972 "Cognitive contours" Nature (London) 238 51-52 Kanizsa G, 1976 "Subjective contours" Scientific American 234 April 48-52 Kennedy J M, Lee H, 1976 "A figure-density hypothesis and illusory contour brightness" Perception 5 387-392 Osgood C E, 1951 Method and Theory in Experimental Psychology (London: Oxford University Press)

p

© 1976 a Pion publication printed in Great Britain

Illusory line linking solid rods.

Perception, 1977, volume 6, pages 601 -602 Illusory line linking solid rods Colin Ware, John M Kennedy Division of Life Sciences, Scarborough Colleg...
230KB Sizes 0 Downloads 0 Views