This article was downloaded by: [New York University] On: 01 August 2015, At: 05:01 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: 5 Howick Place, London, SW1P 1WG

Aging & Mental Health Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/camh20

Improvement of encoding and retrieval in normal and pathological aging with word–picture paradigm a

ab

Rosario Iodice , Juan José G. Meilán a

ab

& Juan Carro

Neurosciences Institute of Castile and Leon, University of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain

b

Psychology Department, University of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain Published online: 06 Jan 2015.

Click for updates To cite this article: Rosario Iodice, Juan José G. Meilán & Juan Carro (2015) Improvement of encoding and retrieval in normal and pathological aging with word–picture paradigm, Aging & Mental Health, 19:10, 940-946, DOI: 10.1080/13607863.2014.995590 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2014.995590

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http:// www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Aging & Mental Health, 2015 Vol. 19, No. 10, 940946, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2014.995590

Improvement of encoding and retrieval in normal and pathological aging with wordpicture paradigm Rosario Iodicea*, Juan Jose G. Meilana,b and Juan Carroa,b a

Neurosciences Institute of Castile and Leon, University of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain; bPsychology Department, University of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain

Downloaded by [New York University] at 05:01 01 August 2015

(Received 23 July 2014; accepted 28 November 2014) Objectives: During the aging process, there is a progressive deficit in the encoding of new information and its retrieval. Different strategies are used in order to maintain, optimize or diminish these deficits in people with and without dementia. One of the classic techniques is paired-associate learning (PAL), which is based on improving the encoding of memories, but it has yet to be used to its full potential in people with dementia. In this study, our aim is to corroborate the importance of PAL tasks as instrumental tools for creating contextual cues, during both the encoding and retrieval phases of memory. Additionally, we aim to identify the most effective form of presenting the related items. Method: Pairs of stimuli were shown to healthy elderly people and to patients with moderate and mild Alzheimer’s disease. The encoding conditions were as follows: word/word, picture/picture, picture/word, and word/picture. Results: Associative cued recall of the second item in the pair shows that retrieval is higher for the word/picture condition in the two groups of patients with dementia when compared to the other conditions, while word/word is the least effective in all cases. Conclusion: These results confirm that PAL is an effective tool for creating contextual cues during both the encoding and retrieval phases in people with dementia when the items are presented using the word/picture condition. In this way, the encoding and retrieval deficit can be reduced in these people. Keywords: Alzheimer; paired-associate learning; cued recall

Introduction The aging process is inevitably associated with memory loss, which becomes particularly relevant when it is associated with the onset of the neuropathological impairment that occurs in Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Many authors have attributed these memory losses to a severe deficit in the development of new associations, thereby making it difficult for older people to combine different memory units into one cohesive unit. AD patients record poor semantic encoding of to-be-learned information, a deficit in the ability to evaluate semantic relationships, and are no longer able to discriminate between two related concepts. This is called the associative deficit hypothesis (Naveh-Benjamin, Hussain, Guez, & Bar-On, 2003). Paired-associate learning (PAL) tasks provide an ideal paradigm for assessing associative memory. This task involves the cued recall of semantically related word pairs, and it is particularly effective in detecting early and preclinical AD (Pike, Rowe, Moss, & Savage, 2008). In other studies, PAL tasks are also used to understand memorial familiarity for pictures and words in patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD. O’Connor and Ally (2010) applied paired-associate stimuli to three groups of participants (healthy elderly people, patients with MCI, and AD patients), varying the mode of incidental presentation of the stimulus: WW (wordword), WP (wordpicture), PW (pictureword), and PP (picturepicture). These researchers were able to confirm the *Corresponding author. Email: [email protected] Ó 2014 Taylor & Francis

benefit produced by the images regarding the words in the subsequent recognition test. However, PAL tasks are used at other times as a strategy for improving encoding processes with semantic contextual cues in people with MCI and AD. Although it has traditionally been posited that patients with dementia of Alzheimer type (DAT) do not improve their memories through the use of cued recall (Monti et al., 1996), other scholars have recently argued that people with MCI and AD may benefit from the use of semantic cues for retrieving words (Carlesimo, Perri, & Caltagirone, 2011). Yet these patients improved only with the introduction of strong cues during both the encoding and retrieval phases (Adam et al., 2007; Johnson, Schmitz, Asthana, Gluck, & Myers, 2008; O’Donnell, Pietrzak, Ellis, Snyder, & Maruff, 2011; Pike et al., 2008). People with moderate and mild dementia are a particular profile of patients in which PAL tasks may have substantial benefits because in these people the rate of memory loss is not elevated. They still maintain the ability to learn and retain some information and skills despite their memory difficulties (Clare & Woods, 2004; Grober, Hall, Sanders, & Lipton, 2008; Grober, Sanders, Hall, & Lipton, 2010). This is particularly true when PAL tasks are used successfully for the generation of associations between the names and faces of people (Brum, Forlenza, & Yassuda, 2009; Tardif & Simard, 2011; Wenisch et al., 2007) or the relationship between faces and moods (Werheid, McDonald, Simmons-Stern, Ally, & Budson, 2011). We contend that

Downloaded by [New York University] at 05:01 01 August 2015

Aging & Mental Health this procedure may also be useful in the encoding and subsequent recall of the association of concepts. In this line of research, there are data indicating that patients with moderate and mild dementia may improve their performance in episodic memory tasks that provide supportive conditions during both the encoding and retrieval phases. It appears necessary to provide retrieval cues in order to obtain the effects of supportive encoding conditions. In particular, this occurs when PAL tasks are used as contextual cues, where the first item of the pair is presented as a cue for the retrieval of the associated item. The cued recall technique aims to enhance spontaneous free recall through the presentation of semantic cues (the first item of the pair), to help both the encoding and the retrieval of the second item in the pair, which is the target of the test. This technique is thought to minimize the effect of impaired memory in cued recall tasks related to moderate dementia and AD (Ivanoiu et al., 2005). However, at the same time it is reported that these patients are not capable of creating a strong semantic association between pairs of words. Patients with moderate dementia and AD have the so-called encoding profile, meaning they can semantically encode verbal materials; however, they do so in a less comprehensive and insufficient manner. Hence it is considered that pairs of words are not the best way of showing the items in order to create contextual cues and, unfortunately, only a few studies are focused on the benefits of PAL tasks as contextual cues during both the encoding and retrieval phases in people with moderate dementia and AD (Tounsi et al., 1999). One possible strategy for improving the procedure may be based on the picture superiority effect. This effect is a consistent empirical finding showing that stimuli presented as pictures are better remembered than stimuli presented as words. This effect has been confirmed in healthy elderly people (Ally et al., 2008), as well as in patients with MCI and AD (Ally, Gold, & Budson, 2009). The explanation for this is that the pictures are more distinctive because of their visual features. Accordingly, in order to corroborate the importance of PAL tasks as effective tools for creating contextual cues during both the encoding and retrieval phases, we introduced pictures in the procedure for pair-associated learning. We sought to determine the best way of presenting the related items showing the combined items under the form of WW, WP, PW, and PP. We have used the overall procedure given by O´Connor and Ally (2010). Nevertheless, in order to convert it into a more natural procedure, we asked the participants to expressly encode the associated pairs they are presented, with our aim being for them to carry out a guided free recall test, instead of a recognition task that is less common in everyday life. We believed that related items in the pair with a visual component, namely, those associations that incorporated an image (WP, PW, PP), could increase both encoding and retrieval performance in cued recall tasks in healthy elderly people, and in patients with moderate and mild AD dementia, when compared to those composed solely of words (WW). According to these considerations, we formulate our first hypothesis, whereby WP, PW, and PP associations have a higher

941

recall rate than WW associations. The second hypothesis is that WP and PW associations have a higher recall rate than WW and PP associations, as these conditions have a double verbalvisual component. In this hierarchical model, as explained by Zeelenberg and Pecher (2003), the conceptual level represented by an image and the lexical and substantive level represented by a word are interconnected, and one activates the other. In this way, the activation of a semantic field (word) allows the image to be contextualized in a semantic network, where less interference is produced during the encoding and retrieval phases. The third hypothesis is that all the above phenomena not only occur in healthy elderly people, but also in people with moderate and mild dementia. This is because patients with pathological aging still maintain the ability to benefit from contextual cues, even in advanced stages of the disease (Adam et al., 2007; Johnson et al., 2008; O’Donnell et al., 2011; Pike et al., 2008). Methods Participants Sixty-four elderly people participated in the study. These participants formed three distinct groups: 22 patients/residents at the State Reference Center for people with Alzheimer’s disease formed the group of moderate dementia (NINCDS-ADRDA, National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association), diagnosed with mild AD and with a Global Deteriorate Scale (GDS) D 4 by a physician at the center. The mean age was 80.43 years (SD D 12.930; age range of 4690) with a Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE weighted by age and years of schooling, Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) mean of 15.57 (SD D 3.673; range 1020). The second group was composed of 21 patients at the State Reference Center for people with AD, diagnosed by a physician at the center as suffering from moderate dementia (NINCDS-ADRDA) with a GDS D 23. The mean age was 82.94 years (SD D 12.407; range 47100), with a MMSE mean of 23.31 (SD D 2.414; range 2125). The families of both groups of patients provided informed consent for their participation in the study. In accordance with the center’s ethical protocol, a report was sent out to each patient’s immediate family or guardian for them to sign and return. The third group was made up of 21 people living in nursing homes who did not have a neurodegenerative pathology, and who were classified as healthy older adults by a physician at the home. The mean age of the group was 80.12 (SD D 7.415; range 6391), with a MMSE mean of 27.29 (SD D 2.024; range 2430). The participants provided their informed consent to participate in the study. We found differences between the three groups in terms of their MMSE values [F (2, 59) D 8.668, p 10), as well as those people with medical backgrounds involving

942

R. Iodice et al.

Table 1. Demographic data. Groups Healthy older adults

Years of education Age MMSE Men Women

AD patients

Means

SD

Means

SD

Means

SD

7.35 80.12 27.29 24% 76%

3.14 7.41 2.02 38% 62%

6.06 82.94 23.31 27% 73%

2.95 12.40 2.41

6.00 80.43 15.57

3.63 12.93 3.67

problems in their communication system or in their ability to read. We confirmed that there were no differences across the three groups in terms of educational level, years of schooling, age, or gender distribution (see Table 1).

Downloaded by [New York University] at 05:01 01 August 2015

Mild dementia

Materials We used a database of 60 pairs of words, from which we randomly obtained 4 lists of 15 pairs for each participant (e.g., eaglemountain, winecheese). As a rule, we used a low average association index for all pairs of stimuli, then selected and matched them based on the factors ‘frequency of use’ and ‘level of association’ from the normative study by Fernandez, Diez, Alonso, and Beato, (2004). Four types of combinations were formed from each list of stimuli: WW, WP, PW, and PP, where the pictures for the WP, PW, and PP combinations were obtained by transforming the actual words in the pairs into their corresponding Snodgrass drawings (Snodgrass & Vanderwart, 1980; see Figure 1). Each pair was shown in four forms (WW, WP, PW, and PP), and presented in PAL tasks, where the first item in the pair (left) favored the encoding and retrieval of the second item in the pair (right). All stimuli were presented using the E-Prime experimental program with 25% in size, displayed on a 1500 screen with a resolution of 1024 £ 768, and placed at a distance of 4800 from the participants. The words were displayed in black against a white background, measuring 3 cm in size.

Procedure Each participant completed four 30-minute sessions, with each one corresponding to a type of encoding (WW, WP,

Figure 1. Combination formed by a word and a picture.

PW, and PP). The sequence of the sessions was randomized for each participant; the sessions were held one week apart from each other. The individual sessions were divided into two phases: one of learning and one of retrieval. In the learning phase, 15 pairs of stimuli were displayed one at a time on the computer screen for 10 seconds, with each one being interspersed with a fixation point displayed in the center of the screen for 1 second. Participants read the words or named the visual stimuli aloud. There was enough time between the two phases to inform the participants, through instructions presented on the computer screen, that the learning task had been completed and that the retrieval task was about to begin. During the retrieval task, the first item in the pair was displayed on the screen, and the participants were asked to remember the associated item. Stimulus presentation in the recovery phase was not subject to a time limit. Scores were obtained assigning 1 point for each correct retrieval, 0.5 for each retrieval by means of a phonetic prompt using the ‘errorless learning’ method (Hopper et al., 2012), and 0 for each retrieval either not performed or performed incorrectly. We thus obtained the recall rate for each participant and for each learning condition performed with PAL tasks.

Statistical analysis With the results obtained, we performed an ANOVA 4 (encoding type: WW, WP, PW, and PP) £ 3 (group: healthy older adults, moderate dementia, and mild dementia AD patients), with the factor-type encoding as withinsubject, and the factor group as inter-subject. Based on the factors recording significant differences (p < 0.05), we conducted Bonferroni post hoc tests on the comparison of pairs.

Results Table 2 presents the descriptive data. The multivariate contrast recorded significant differences due to the main encoding factor [F (3, 59) D 44.525, p < .001]. As regards the post hoc tests (see Figure 2), the WP encoding obtained significantly higher recall rates when compared to the WW encoding (p < .001), PW (p < .001), and PP (p < .001). The WW encoding had a significantly lower performance with respect to PW (p < .001) and PP

Aging & Mental Health

943

Table 2. Group recall rates of the second item in the pair in cued recall PAL tasks. Groups Healthy older adults

Downloaded by [New York University] at 05:01 01 August 2015

Word/word Word/picture Picture/word Picture/picture Means

Mild dementia

12

SD

Means

SD

Means

SD

Means

SD

9.64 11.92 9.59 11.00 10.54

2.49 2.37 2.41 2.67 2.48

7.33 10.38 9.11 8.50 8.83

2.67 2.49 2.71 3.02 2.72

5.15 7.75 6.25 6.45 6.40

1.88 2.06 2.53 2.26 2.18

7.34 9.98 8.28 8.61

2.97 2.87 2.93 3.22

Groups Healthy Older Adults

10 8

Moderate Dementia

6 4

Means

Means

(p < .001). Finally, the PW and PP encodings recorded no significant differences. We also found significant differences due to the group factor [F (2, 61) D 19.55, p < .001]. The group of healthy older adults recorded a significantly higher success rate when compared to the groups of patients with moderate dementia (p < .005) and with mild dementia (p < .001). Furthermore, patients with moderate dementia recorded a significantly higher success rate compared to mild dementia patients (p < .001). Finally, we found significant interaction effects between the two factors [F (6, 120) D 2.254, p < .001]. The group of patients with mild AD dementia manifested a significantly lower performance in the four types of encoding (see Figure 2) in comparison to patients with moderate dementia and the healthy older adults group (p < .001). The group of patients with moderate dementia recorded a significantly lower performance in WW (p < .001) and PP (p < .001) compared to the healthy older adults, but was not different in the WP and PW encoding types. If we consider the performance of each group in the different types of encoding, we find that the three groups performed better in the WP encoding condition compared to WW (all groups, p < .001) and PW (all groups, p < .001). The WP encoding condition produced more efficient recall with respect to PP solely in the groups with dementia (moderate dementia, p < .001; mild dementia, p < .005). The PP encoding condition proved to be 14

AD patients

Mild dementia

2 0

Figure 2. Group recall rates in PAL tasks for different encoding types.

beneficial for all three groups when compared to WW (all groups, p < .005), but only beneficial for the group of healthy older adults when compared to PW (p < .001). Furthermore, the PP condition did not generate any advantages in the group with mild or moderate dementia when compared to WP and PW. The PW encoding condition was beneficial only for the group with moderate dementia when it was compared with WW (p < .001), while it did not produce any significant advantage for the groups with mild dementia and the healthy older adults.

Conclusions In general, as we assumed in our first hypothesis, there is an improvement in the recall of information when a picture is part of the associated pair, as compared to the use solely of written words. This is a further finding to support recent studies that argue that visual recall cues help to restore the memory of learnt associations. Nevertheless, we have shown here that the effective use of the advantageous properties of images during the encoding and retrieval processes requires the pictures to be presented in a particular way. The most efficient encoding type is when the first item in the pair is a word and the second item in the pair is a picture. This is because images are a privileged category of stimuli that enrich the verbal material that is being learned, by means of double encoding via both semantic/ conceptual and visual perception. Images favor information encoding processes, allowing for deeper processing, generating less interference in memory, and thereby favoring the discrimination between items (Curran & Doyle, 2011). The familiarity of the associations studied is greatly enhanced if a word (the first item in the pair) previously activates a semantic network that will more closely connect a visual representation (Ally & Budson, 2007). In other words, the word generates an activation effect that allows for a quicker and easier retrieval of the picture studied (Zeelenberg & Pecher, 2003) because the word previously activates a semantic network helping to closely converge the memory with its visual representation. As expected, the groups varied significantly depending on the degree of cognitive impairment. People with mild AD recorded a significantly lower performance in all circumstances with respect to the other groups, but the group of patients with moderate dementia appears to have

Downloaded by [New York University] at 05:01 01 August 2015

944

R. Iodice et al.

made notable use of the benefits of the WP and PW paradigm, being no different to the healthy older adults group. We may conclude, therefore, that the semantic and visual coding represented by the WP and PW paradigm is particularly useful for creating an optimal cued recall response in people with moderate dementia. This is relevant mainly because it is believed that only images favor both the coding and retrieval phases of learned material, and stimulate the memory in people with dementia (Ally, McKeever, Waring, & Budson, 2009). The results here would appear to defend the idea that the items need to be shown in a double encoding form (WP paradigm) to people with moderate dementia in order to create an effective contextual cue. The use of images to stimulate memory in older people is well known, being practiced in the majority of nursing homes around the world. It has traditionally been considered that images are more efficiently processed through both verbal and visual encoding channels (Paivio, 1991; Sadoski & Paivio, 2004), as they have a dual sensory-semantic system of representation and encoding (Nelson, McEvoy, & Schreiber, 2004), which thereby enriches the information of the stimulus (Ally et al., 2009). This association activates categorization processes, generating a clearer and more precise conceptual representation of the information that ultimately suffers from less interference. Consequently, this increases the likelihood of success in the memory phase (Cherry, Hawley, Jackson, & Volaufova, 2008). Nevertheless, images help to create more effective encoding and improve semantic processes, generating a deeper encoding of information that provides a familiar structure of items that are easily managed by memory (Lovseth & Atchley, 2010). Accordingly, the interferences generated when new information is acquired and retrieved are significantly reduced, ensuring an organization that promotes the associations between the items, giving the information greater stability during the learning process, and establishing a connection between previous and recent learning (Campos, PerezFabello, & Camino, 2010). According to some authors, words together with visual stimuli activate a pre-semantic level that interacts with episodic and semantic systems, favoring perceptual priming, which is relatively well preserved in patients with dementia (Garrard, Lambon Ralph, Patterson, Pratt, & Hodges, 2005; Heindel, Salmon, & Butters, 1990; Job & Tenconi, 2002). The formation of perceptual priming involves the words and images in a similar process, because the overlapping of codes allows for access to a common conceptual memory storage area (Kazmerski & Friedman, 1997), thereby generating deeper associations and improving memory retrieval. Nonetheless, double verbalvisual encoding is not always the core reason for the improvement in the test’s performance. When we have followed the sequence of picture followed by a word, only those patients with a moderate impairment benefit from the use of these recall cues compared to the use of written words. Curiously enough, the PW format tends to be used in many nursing homes where an open display is made of a drawing followed by its lexical association (e.g., a flower and the

name of the patient ‘Rose’). Yet this format does not seems to be of any great use to people with severe cognitive impairment or to older adults without such impairment. The pictures themselves do not trigger the appropriate semantic network, often interfering with the activation of the word that appears in second place. This means that we cannot fully confirm our second hypothesis on the similar effectiveness of the PW and WP formats in people with dementia. These findings differ from those reported by O´Connor and Ally (2010) for a recognition task. These scholars found that two pictures constituted the condition that best assisted recognition, with wordface (WF) pairs recording the worst performance. Their findings seem to suggest that the degree of perceptive familiarity and distinctiveness of the picture shown in the test is the key aspect for recognition. In our study, the PP condition is an effective procedure for healthy elderly people, but not for those with cognitive impairment. Healthy people are able to use the distinctiveness of the pictures, whereas those with cognitive impairment cannot use this resource. The impaired management of spatial memories has recently been reported by Toepper et al. (2014). Finally, we consider that this line of research should be examined further in order to contribute to the development of neuropsychological rehabilitation techniques designed to help healthy elderly adults and those with neurodegenerative diseases maintain the ability to memorize new information and/or develop procedures to restore lost learning. Among others, we believe that this line of research can be used in conjunction with other cognitive techniques based on the so-called picture superiority effect (Allyet al., 2009; Curran & Doyle, 2011) and cued recall PAL tasks (Boudreaux, Cherry, Elliott, & Hicks, 2011; Cherry et al., 2012), where the encoding and retrieval of information can be enhanced in healthy elderly people (Laeng, Øvervoll, & Ole Steinsvik, 2007) and people with mild dementia (Ally et al., 2008; Cabeza, Anderson, Locantore, & McIntosh, 2002) or AD (Hussey, Smolinsky, Piryatinsky, Budson, & Ally, 2011; Joubert et al., 2010). Addressing the importance of double encoding as regards the distinctiveness of pictures would require enlarging the sample used. It would also be expedient to consider the role of interference in the absence of wholly positive results in certain conditions involving the use of pictures. Finally, there is a need to confirm the influence that institutionalization has on these results in comparison with non-institutionalized individuals. Acknowledgements We would like to express gratitude to the CRE Alzheimer’s Salamanca-IMSERSO; Government of Spain for the contract for research collaboration between The Neurosciences Institute of Castile and Leon and The National Reference Centre of Alzheimer Disease.

Disclosure statement No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Aging & Mental Health Funding This work was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation [grant number MICINN, # BFU2010-17754].

Downloaded by [New York University] at 05:01 01 August 2015

References Adam, S., Van der Linden, M., Ivanoiu, A., Juillerat, A.-C., Bechet, S., & Salmon, E. (2007). Optimization of encoding specificity for the diagnosis of early AD: The RI-48 task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 477487. doi:10.1080/13803390600775339. Ally, B.A., & Budson, A.E. (2007). The worth of pictures: Using high density event-related potentials to understand the memorial power of pictures and the dynamics of recognition memory. NeuroImage, 35(1), 378395. Ally, B.A., Gold, C.A., & Budson, A.E. (2009). The picture superiority effect in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 595598. Ally, B.A., McKeever, J.D., Waring, J.D., & Budson, A.E. (2009). Preserved frontal memorial processing for pictures in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 20442055. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. neuropsychologia.2009.03.015 Ally, B.A., Waring, J.D., Beth, E.H., McKeever, J.D., Milberg, W.P., & Budson, A.E. (2008). Aging memory for pictures: Using high-density event-related potentials to understand the effect of aging on the picture superiority effect. Neuropsychologia, 46(2), 679689. doi: http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.011 Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., & Brown, G.K. (1996). BDI-II. Beck depression inventory-II. Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Boudreaux, E.O., Cherry, K.E., Elliott, E.M., & Hicks, J.L. (2011). Effects of distraction and pictorial illustration on memory for countries in older adults with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Experimental Aging Research, 37(3), 293309. doi: http://dx. doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2011.568816 Brum, P.S., Forlenza, O.V., & Yassuda, M.S. (2009). Cognitive training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 3(2), 124131. Cabeza, R., Anderson, N.D., Locantore, J.K., & McIntosh, A.R. (2002). Aging gracefully: Compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. NeuroImage, 17(3), 13941402. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1280 Campos, A., Perez-Fabello, M.J., & Camino, E. (2010). Efficacy of the keyword mnemonic method in adults. Psicothema, 22 (4), 752757. Carlesimo, G.A., Perri, R., & Caltagirone, C. (2011). Category cued recall following controlled encoding as a neuropsychological tool in the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: A review of the evidence. Neuropsychology Review, 21(1), 5465. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-010-9153-7 Cherry, K., Hawley, K., Jackson, E., & Volaufova, J. (2008). Pictorial superiority effects in oldest-old people. Memory, 16(7), 728741. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 09658210802215534.Pictorial Cherry, K.E., Silva Brown, J., Jackson Walker, E., Smitherman, E.A., Boudreaux, E.O., Volaufova, J., & Michal Jazwinski, S. (2012). Semantic encoding enhances the pictorial superiority effect in the oldest-old. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 19(12), 319337. doi: http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1080/13825585.2011.619645 Clare, L., & Woods, R.T. (2004). Cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation for people with early-stage Alzheimer’s disease: A review. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 14(4), 385401. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602010443000074

945

Curran, T., & Doyle, J. (2011). Picture superiority doubly dissociates the ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(5), 12471262. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21464 Fernandez, A., Diez, E., Alonso, M.A., & Beato, M.S. (2004). Free-association norms for the Spanish names of the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers : A Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc., 36(3), 577583. Folstein, M., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). “Minimental state” a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198. Garrard, P., Lambon Ralph, M.A., Patterson, K., Pratt, K.H., & Hodges, J.R. (2005). Semantic feature knowledge and picture naming in dementia of Alzheimer’s type: A new approach. Brain and Language, 93(1), 7994. doi: http://dx. doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.08.003 Grober, E., Hall, C., Sanders, A.E., & Lipton, R.B. (2008). Free and cued selective reminding distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from vascular dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 56(5), 944946. doi: http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01652.x Grober, E., Sanders, A.E., Hall, C., & Lipton, R.B. (2010). Free and cued selective reminding identifies very mild dementia in primary care. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 24(3), 284290. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ WAD.0b013e3181cfc78b Heindel, W.C., Salmon, D.P., & Butters, N. (1990). Pictorial priming and cued recall in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 13(2), 282295. Hopper, T., Bourgeois, M., Pimentel, J., Qualls, C.D., Hickey, E., Frymark, T., & Schooling, T. (2012). An evidence-based systematic review on cognitive interventions for individuals with dementia. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology / American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. doi: http:// dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0137) Hussey, E.P., Smolinsky, J.G., Piryatinsky, I., Budson, A.E., & Ally, B.A. (2011). Using mental imagery to improve memory in patients with Alzheimer disease: Trouble generating or remembering the mind’s eye? Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 2(26), 124134. doi: http://dx.doi. org/10.1097/WAD.0b013e31822e0f73 Ivanoiu, A., Adam, S., Van der Linden, M., Salmon, E., Juillerat, A.-C., Mulligan, R., & Seron, X. (2005). Memory evaluation with a new cued recall test in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Neurology, 252(1), 4755. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00415-0050597 Job, R., & Tenconi, E. (2002). Naming pictures at no cost: Asymmetries in picture and word conditional naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 790794. doi: http://dx. doi.org/10.3758/BF03196336 Johnson, S.C., Schmitz, T.W., Asthana, S., Gluck, M.A., & Myers, C. (2008). Associative learning over trials activates the hippocampus in healthy elderly but not mild cognitive impairment. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 15(2), 129145. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13825580601139444 Joubert, S., Brambati, S.M., Ansado, J., Barbeau, E.J., Felician, O., Didic, M., . . . Kergoat, M.-J. (2010). The cognitive and neural expression of semantic memory impairment in mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 48(4), 978988. doi: http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.019 Kazmerski, V.A., & Friedman, D. (1997). Old/new differences in direct and indirect memory tests using pictures and words in within- and cross-form conditions: Event-related potential and behavioral measures. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 5(4), 255272. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ S0926-6410(97)00004-9

Downloaded by [New York University] at 05:01 01 August 2015

946

R. Iodice et al.

Laeng, B., Øvervoll, M., & Ole Steinsvik, O. (2007). Remembering 1500 pictures: The right hemisphere remembers better than the left. Brain and Cognition, 63(2), 136144. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.10.009 Lovseth, K., & Atchley, R.A. (2010). Examining lateralized semantic access using pictures. Brain and Cognition, 72(2), 202209. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.016 Monti, L.A., Gabrieli, J.D. E., Reminger, S.L., Rinaldi, J.A., Wilson, R.S., & Fleischman, D.A. (1996). Differential effects of aging and Alzheimer’s disease on conceptual implicit and explicit memory. Neuropsychology, 10, 101112. Naveh-Benjamin, M., Hussain, Z., Guez, J., & Bar-On, M. (2003). Adult age differences in episodic memory: Further support for an associative-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 826837. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/02787393.29.5.826 Nelson, D.L., McEvoy, C.L., & Schreiber, T.A. (2004). The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers: A Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc., 36(3), 402407. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03195588 O’Connor, M.K., & Ally, B.A. (2010). Using stimulus form change to understand memorial familiarity for pictures and words in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 48(7), 20682074. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.027 O’Donnell, J., Pietrzak, R.H., Ellis, K.C., Snyder, P.J., & Maruff, P. (2011). Understanding failure of visual paired associate learning in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(10), 10691078. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2011.596821 Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal Of Psychology, 45(3), 255287. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0084295 Pike, K.E., Rowe, C.C., Moss, S.A., & Savage, G. (2008). Memory profiling with paired associate learning in Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, and healthy aging. Neuropsychology, 22(6), 718728. doi: http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/a0013050

Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2004). A dual coding theoretical model of reading. In R. R. Ruddell, & N. J. Unrau, (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (5th ed.), (pp. 13291362). Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Snodgrass, J.G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Learning and Memory, 6(2), 174215. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.6.2.174 Tardif, S., & Simard, M. (2011). Cognitive stimulation programs in healthy elderly: A review. International Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 2011, 378934. doi: http://dx.doi.org/ 10.4061/2011/378934 Toepper, M., Markowitsch, H.J., Gebhardt, H., Beblo, Th., Bauer, E., Woermann, F.G., Driessen, M., Sammer, G. (2014). The impact of age on prefrontal cortex integrity during spatial working memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 59, 157168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.04.020 Tounsi, H., Deweer, B., Ergis, A.M., Van der Linden, M., Pillon, B., Michon, A., & Dubois, B. (1999). Sensitivity to semantic cuing: An index of episodic memory dysfunction in early Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 13(1), 3846. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ 00002093-199903000-00006 Wenisch, E., Cantegreil-Kallen, I., De Rotrou, J., Garrigue, P., Moulin, F., Batouche, F., . . . Rigaud, A.S. (2007). Cognitive stimulation intervention for elders with mild cognitive impairment compared with normal aged subjects: Preliminary results. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 19, 316322. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF03324708 Werheid, K., McDonald, R.S., Simmons-Stern, N., Ally, B.A., Budson, A.E. (2011). Familiar smiling faces in Alzheimer’s disease: Understanding the positivity-related recognition bias. Neuropsychologia, 49, 29352940. doi:10.1016/j. neuropsychologia.2011.06.022. Zeelenberg, R., & Pecher, D. (2003). Evidence for long-term cross-language repetition priming in conceptual implicit memory tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 8094. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03) 00020-2

Improvement of encoding and retrieval in normal and pathological aging with word-picture paradigm.

During the aging process, there is a progressive deficit in the encoding of new information and its retrieval. Different strategies are used in order ...
207KB Sizes 0 Downloads 6 Views