Alcohol and Alcoholism Vol. 50, No. 2, pp. 132–139, 2015 Advance Access Publication 1 December 2014

doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agu084

Increased Alcohol Consumption in Urocortin 3 Knockout Mice Is Unaffected by Chronic Inflammatory Pain Monique L. Smith, Ju Li and Andrey E. Ryabinin* Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health & Science University, 3181 SW Sam Jackson Park Road L470, Portland, OR 97239-3098, USA *Corresponding author: Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health & Science University, 3181 SW Sam Jackson Park Road L470, Portland, OR 97239-3098, USA. Tel.: +1-503-494-2060; Fax: +1-503-494-6877; E-mail: [email protected] (Received 14 July 2014; in revised form 30 October 2014; accepted 3 November 2014) Abstract — Aims: Stress neurocircuitry may modulate the relationship between alcohol drinking and chronic pain. The corticotropinreleasing factor (CRF) system is crucial for regulation of stress responses. The current study aimed to elucidate the role of the endogenous CRF ligand Urocortin 3 (Ucn3) in the relationship between alcohol drinking behavior and chronic pain using a genetic approach. Methods: Ucn3 (KO) and wildtype (WT) littermates were subjected to a 24-h access drinking procedure prior to and following induction of chronic inflammatory pain. Results: Ucn3 KO mice displayed significantly increased ethanol intake and preference compared with WT across the time course. There were no long-term effects of chronic pain on alcohol drinking behavior, regardless of genotype, nor any evidence for alcohol-induced analgesia. Conclusion: The increased drinking in Ucn3 KO supports a role for this peptide in alcohol-related behavior. These data suggest the necessity for more research exploring the relationship between alcohol drinking, chronic pain and the CRF system in rodent models.

INTRODUCTION

© The Author 2014. Medical Council on Alcohol and Oxford University Press. All rights reserved

Downloaded from by guest on November 15, 2015

Alcohol Use Disorders (AUDs) affect 140 million people worldwide (World Health Organization) and are often comorbid with other pathologies, such as chronic pain. Approximately one in five people suffer from unrelenting pain for which there is no truly successful treatment (International Association on the Study of Pain), and chronic pain patients often report use of alcohol for pain relief (Brennan et al., 2005; Riley and King, 2009). Conversely, chronic pain disorders can develop following alcohol dependence (Katon et al., 1985). Furthermore, it has been demonstrated that there is a bidirectional relationship between family history of AUDs and chronic pain, such that the occurrence of one of these disorders can predict family occurrence of the other (Katon et al., 1985; Stewart et al., 1995; Goldberg et al., 1999). Despite compelling evidence for a relationship between pain and alcohol disorders, this connection has not been thoroughly investigated in preclinical models. A wide body of research suggests that alcohol-induced neuroadaptations within stress regulatory systems contribute to the transition into alcoholism (Heilig and Koob, 2007; Breese et al., 2011; Ryabinin et al., 2012; Koob, 2013). Similarly, other pathological states including chronic pain are characterized by functionally altered stress neurocircuitry (Gianoulakis et al., 2003; Ji et al., 2007; Edwards and Koob, 2010; Rouwette et al., 2012). Therefore it is possible that aberrant stress neurocircuitry may modulate the relationship between alcohol drinking and chronic pain behaviors (see Egli et al., 2012). This modulation may be traced to the corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) system, which plays an important role in the regulation of stress, AUDs and pain disorders. Within the CRF family, there exist multiple endogenous ligands (CRF and the Urocortins: Ucn1, Ucn2 and Ucn3) as well as multiple receptors (CRFR1, CRFR2) and the CRF binding protein (CRFBP). Ucn1 and CRF can bind to both CRFR1 and CRFR2, while Ucn2 and Ucn3 are primarily selective for CRFR2 (Lewis et al., 2001; Reyes et al., 2001). The ability of the multiple components within this system to act in concert to regulate behavior is a process that is not fully understood (Gravanis and

Margioris, 2005; Giardino and Ryabinin, 2012; Janssen and Kozicz, 2013; Zorrilla et al., 2014). Nevertheless, previous research has demonstrated that central injection of Ucn3 decreases alcohol drinking in mice in limited access procedures (Sharpe and Phillips, 2009; Lowery et al., 2010). Additionally, drinking is decreased when Ucn3 is administered into the central nucleus of the amygdala of ethanol (EtOH)-dependent rats (Funk and Koob, 2007). These studies demonstrate that exogenously administered Ucn3, a peptide targeting CRFR2, is capable of regulating alcohol consumption. However, interpretation of the effects of exogenously administered Ucn3 is difficult, as these effects may be endogenously mediated by Ucn1, Ucn2, Ucn3 or even CRF acting on these receptors. This caveat necessitates investigation of the role of Ucn3 in alcohol consumption using non-pharmacological tools, for example, using transgenic or knockout technologies. Another important function of the CRF system relates to pain and analgesia (McNally and Akil, 2002; Fu and Neugebauer, 2008; Ji and Neugebauer, 2008; Edwards et al., 2012). It has even been theorized that Ucn3 may be responsible for CRFR2 mediated pain inhibition within the amygdala, due to the lack of evidence for other endogenous ligands within this area (see Rouwette et al., 2012). Neuroanatomical evidence also points to potential involvement of Ucn3 in pain behaviors. Specifically, Ucn3 neurons and fibers are located in the rostral perifornical area of the hypothalamus, the periaqueductal gray and the posterior bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Lewis et al., 2001; Li et al., 2002; Wittmann et al., 2009). All of these areas are involved in aspects of pain transmission and processing or innervate such areas (e.g.: amygdala). These converging lines of evidence highlight the potential involvement of Ucn3 in both alcohol and pain related processing. Thus, we aimed to elucidate the role of endogenous Ucn3 in the relationship between alcohol drinking behavior and chronic pain. To accomplish this aim, we examined chronic alcohol drinking during concomitant chronic inflammatory pain in Ucn3 KO mice with the hypothesis that Ucn3 KO would increase drinking and this effect may be modulated by the presence of chronic pain.

Increased alcohol consumption in Urocortin 3 KO

MATERIALS AND METHODS Animals Adult male and female Ucn3 knockout (KO) mice and wild type (WT) littermates were used in all experiments. These single gene mutant mice were generated by targeted deletion, as described in detail elsewhere (Li et al., 2007). Breeding pairs were generously provided by Drs. Wylie Vale and Mark Huising from the Salk Institute (La Jolla, California). These mice were generated on C57BL/6J × 129s background and backcrossed onto the C57BL/6J (B6) background for four generations. KO and WT mice were generated by crossing of heterozygous animals. Mice were genotyped and weaned at 28–32 days and isosexually housed 2–5 per cage. Between 30 and 33 weeks of age, mice were individually housed and transferred to the experimental room with a 12/12 reverse light/dark cycle for a 7-day acclimation period prior to the initiation of the experiments. All housing rooms were temperature and humidity controlled and food and water were available ad libitum. All protocols were approved by the Oregon Health & Science University animal care and use committee and performed within the National Institutes for Health Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, as well as the Guidelines for the Care and use of Mammals and Behavioral Research.

Mechanical testing and chronic inflammatory pain Following acclimation to the experimental room, mice were habituated to the testing area for 40 min on 2 separate days. In addition, on each testing day, mice were allowed to acclimate to the testing rack for 20 min. The rack was located on a large table adjacent to home cages, within the experimental room. All testing and manipulation occurred 2 h into the dark cycle under a dimly lit red light. Following habituation, but prior to EtOH access, mice were tested for basal responses to punctate mechanical stimuli according to the up-down method (Chaplan et al., 1994). Briefly, the plantar surface of the animals left hindpaw was stimulated with a series of eight von Frey filaments (bending force ranging from 0.01 to 2 g). A response was defined as lifting or shaking of the stimulated paw. Beginning with the 0.4 g filament, a lack of response led to testing with a heavier filament (up), and in the presence of a response, a lighter filament (down)

was attempted. This was repeated for a maximum of 4 filaments following the first response. In order to determine whether EtOH drinking altered mechanical thresholds, mice were tested again after 2 days of 10% EtOH access. To induce chronic inflammatory pain, on the fourth day of 10% EtOH, mice were removed from their cages, lightly anesthetized with 1–2% isoflurane and injected with 10 µl Complete Freunds Adjuvant (CFA; 1 mg Mycobacterium tuberculosis (H37Ra, ATCC 25177)/ml of emulsion in 85% paraffin oil and 15% mannide manooleate – Sigma) in the intraplantar surface of the left hindpaw, which is known to reliably induce long lasting pain (Ren and Dubner, 1999). Induction of anesthesia and injection of CFA took 1–2 min, and recovery of mobility occurred within a minute post injection. Mice were then returned to their cages with access to EtOH and water. Following induction of pain, mice were tested weekly for mechanical von Frey thresholds for a total of 28 days, or four tests post-CFA. Statistical analysis EtOH consumption in ml was converted to grams (based on concentration) and divided by the animal’s body weight to give daily intake scores expressed in grams per kilogram (g/kg). EtOH preference was calculated by dividing EtOH consumption (ml) by total fluid consumption ml (EtOH ml + water ml). Total intake is expressed in milliliters per kilogram (ml/kg). Alcohol intake, preference and total intake were analyzed by repeated measures ANOVA examining sex (male, female) and genotype (WT, KO) as the between subject factors, and day or concentration as within subject factors. Blood ethanol content (BEC) and all basal mechanical thresholds were compared via one-way ANOVA. Weekly mechanical testing was also compared via repeated measures ANOVA with sex and genotype as the between subject factors and test session as within subject factor. For all analyses, significance threshold was set at P < 0.05. Data are expressed as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). Unless mentioned, all other effects and interactions were not significant (P > 0.05). RESULTS Body weights Male mice weighed significantly more than female mice, and all bodyweights increased across time according to repeated measures ANOVA. Thus, there was a significant effect of sex (F(1,31) = 19.69, P < 0.0001) and day (F(34,1054) = 10.26, P = 0.0032). There were no other significant effects or interactions (Table 1). EtOH intake and preference As expected, EtOH intake was higher in females, and intake increased for both sexes as concentrations increased over time. Interestingly, both male and female KO mice drank slightly more than their respective WT controls (Fig. 1A and B). The effect of genotype was not confirmed when examined using day as the within subject factor, but was significant when examined using concentration as the within subject factor. Thus, a repeated measures ANOVA analyzing all days of the experiment only revealed an effect of sex (F(1,31) = 24.429, P < 0.0001), and day (F(34,1054) = 43.709, P < 0.0001), as well as an interaction between sex and day (F(34,1054) = 4.114,

Downloaded from by guest on November 15, 2015

24 h access 2-bottle choice EtOH solutions (v/v) were prepared in tap water from 95% ethyl alcohol. During the acclimation period, the mice were allowed 24-h access to two 25 ml glass cylinder bottles with metal sipper tubes (containing water) on either side of the cage, with food evenly spread across the cage top. Mice underwent 35 days of EtOH drinking, during which they received 24 h access to two bottles: one containing tap water and one containing increasing concentrations of EtOH (6–20%) dissolved in tap water. Six percent EtOH was available for 3 days, followed by access to 10% for 18 days and 20% for 14 days. All bottles were introduced and fluid levels were recorded on a daily basis between 1000 and 1100 h (2 h into the dark cycle). The locations of the bottles on the cages (left vs. right) were alternated daily to avoid the potential confound of an inherent side preference. All mice were sacrificed for BEC 2 h into the dark cycle on the final day of 20% drinking (immediately following the final von Frey test).

133

134

Smith et al.

P < 0.0001). Yet a repeated measures ANOVA examining intake across the three concentrations of EtOH indicated an effect of sex (F(1,31) = 27.455, P < 0.001), concentration Table 1. Bodyweights of experimental animals Mean body weight GT

n Sex

Pre-CFA

Post-CFA

WT Male Female

7 10

26.76 ± 0.96 21.57 ± 0.75

26.10 ± 1.33 23.59 ± 0.91

Male Female

7 11

25.29 ± 0.53 22.00 ± 0.63

27.7 ± 1.21 24.13 ± 0.41

KO

Mice were weighed prior to acclimation to the room and following the completion of the final von Frey test on Week 5. Overall, females weighed less than males. There were no significant effects with genotype or interactions with genotype.

(F(2,62) = 126.631, P < 0.0001) and genotype (F(1,31) = 5.523, P = 0.041), and an interaction between sex and concentration (F(2,62) = 11.9, P < 0.0001). As with intake, EtOH preference was significantly increased in females compared with males and Ucn3 KO compared with WT. Additionally, preference increased in all groups across day and concentrations (Fig. 2A and B). When examining all days of the study, a repeated measures ANOVA revealed significant effects of sex (F(1,31) = 8.155, P = 0.0076), genotype (F(1,31) = 19.803, P = 0.0001) and day (F(34,1054) = 8.45, P < 0.0001). Additionally, a when examining these variables across the three concentrations, a repeated measures ANOVA revealed a main effect of sex (F(1,31) = 19.54, P < 0.0001), genotype (F(1,31) = 9.13, P = 0.005) and concentration (F(2,62) = 9.85, P < 0.0002) on ethanol preference, but no interactions between these variables. All groups demonstrated a notable decrease in alcohol intake and preference for the 24 h following CFA injection (Figs 1 and 2), which likely contributed to the significant

Downloaded from by guest on November 15, 2015

Fig. 1. Twenty-four hour access alcohol intake (g/kg) of WT and KO mice across the 5 week timecourse. Ethanol concentration is represented by the solid horizontal lines below the x-axis. Weekly mechanical test sessions are represented by vertical dotted lines. (A) Male KO mice (n = 7) drank significantly more alcohol than male WT mice (n = 7), primarily at the 10% concentration of EtOH. (B) Female KO (n = 11) mice drank significantly more than female WT (n = 10) mice, primarily at the 20% concentration. Additionally, all female mice drank at higher levels than male mice of both genotypes.

Fig. 2. Alcohol preference of WT and KO mice across the 5 week timecourse. Ethanol concentration is represented by the solid horizontal lines below the x-axis. Weekly mechanical test sessions are represented by vertical dotted lines. (A) Male KO mice (n = 7) had a significantly higher preference for alcohol over water than male WT mice, primarily at the higher concentrations of EtOH. (B) Female KO mice (n = 11) also demonstrated a higher preference from alcohol than female WT mice (n = 10). In general, female had a slightly higher preference for alcohol than male mice.

Increased alcohol consumption in Urocortin 3 KO

135

Fig. 3. Total fluid intake (ml/kg) of WT and KO mice across the 5 week timecourse. Ethanol concentration is represented by the solid horizontal lines below the x-axis. Weekly mechanical test sessions are represented by vertical dotted lines. (A) Male KO (n = 7) and WT (n = 7) mice drank similar amounts throughout the timecourse. (B) Female KO (n = 11) and WT (n = 10) mice also drank similar amounts throughout the timecourse.

effect of day on this measure. However, this effect disappeared by 48 h, suggesting that chronic inflammatory pain does not alter alcohol intake or preference for alcohol.

Downloaded from by guest on November 15, 2015

Total fluid intake There was a slight decrease in fluid intake over the course of the study, resulting in a main effect of day (F(34,1054) = 9.964, P < 0.0001). There were no other significant main effects or interactions when comparing the total intake levels of the groups (Fig. 3). Blood ethanol content It appeared that male KO had higher BECs than WT on the final test day, but this difference was not significant according to a one-way ANOVA including genotype and sex (F(3,34) = 1.75, P = 0.177; Fig. 4). Mechanical sensitivity Although male KO appeared to have higher mechanical thresholds than all other groups at baseline, this difference did not reach significance according to an ANOVA comparing genotype and sex (F(3,34) = 2.5, P = 0.078) (Fig. 5A). This tendency disappeared following access to EtOH, such that a one-way ANOVA demonstrated no differences in mechanical sensitivity according to genotype or sex (F(3,34) = 0.95, P = 0.44) (Fig. 5B). CFA induced a similar level of pain in all groups and though pain dissipated over time, it was not altered by access to alcohol, suggesting a lack of EtOH-induced analgesia in all groups. In fact, a repeated measures ANOVA comparing the weekly test sessions revealed significant main effect of week (F(5,155) = 35.03, P < 0.0001), but there were no other main effects or interactions (Fig. 6). DISCUSSION In this study we provide the first evidence for the role of endogenous Ucn3 in long-term, moderate alcohol consumption. In general, Ucn3 KO mice had a significantly higher

Fig. 4. Plasma ethanol concentrations. Blood ethanol concentration 4 h into the final day of alcohol drinking. There were no significant differences according to sex or genotype.

preference for alcohol and drank more than WT controls throughout the experiment. However, the pattern of this effect seemed to differ between the sexes. Although there was no statistical interaction between sex and concentration, it appears that males had a higher preference for alcohol during Weeks 2–4, or the last week of 10%, and first week of 20% access, whereas females preferred alcohol most strongly for the entire period of 20% access. These results present another line of evidence supporting the role of Ucn3 in drinking behavior. Previous research has shown that injection of Ucn3 can decrease drinking behavior in non-dependent mice (Sharpe and Phillips, 2009; Lowery et al., 2010) and in alcohol-dependent rats (Funk et al., 2006). These previous studies used pharmacological techniques that

136

Smith et al.

Fig. 5. Baseline mechanical thresholds prior to- and post-EtOH. The mean average of all groups is represented by the horizontal dotted line. (A) Prior to alcohol access, there were no significant differences when comparing male and female Ucn3 KO and WT mice. (B) Following 1 week of EtOH access mechanical thresholds remained consistent, and there were no significant differences between groups.

Downloaded from by guest on November 15, 2015

Fig. 6. Weekly mechanical thresholds following chronic pain. Post-EtOH baselines are represented by timepoint zero on the x-axis and the horizontal dotted line. CFA injection occurred following baseline testing and is represented by the vertical dotted line. (A) Male KO (n = 7) and WT (n = 7) did not significantly differ throughout the timecourse. CFA induced a significant decrease in mechanical sensitivity, which persisted for the entire 5 week timecourse. (B) Similarly, female KO (n = 11) and WT (n = 11) both demonstrated a significant decrease in mechanical sensitivity following CFA injection (although this effect was not significant by Week 5).

cannot delineate which endogenous CRF-related peptide is responsible for effects on alcohol drinking, whereas the current study investigated the role of endogenous Ucn3 in alcohol drinking using the genetic deletion approach. Although studies utilizing genetic mutations must be carefully considered, the direction of effects within our study and previous pharmacological studies are in agreement, which strongly suggests the observed effects are not due to developmental compensations. The current findings also need to be considered in relation to the effect of this mutation on other phenotypes. Although no gross changes in these animals have been reported, it has been found that these mice can display increased resistance to glucose intolerance, hyperinsulinemia and hepatic steatosis

following prolonged exposure to high fat diets (Li et al., 2007). However, since our mice were not exposed to a high fat diet, it is unlikely that these effects explain the observed drinking phenotype. Furthermore, an independently generated line of Ucn3 KO mice did not display any alterations except for an enhanced retention of social discrimination memory (Deussing et al., 2010). The latter caveat does not apply to the current studies, due to the fact that our mice were drinking in isolation. Further considerations are that the current findings in the Ucn3 KO mice could be explained by differences in body weight, initial sensitivity to alcohol or alcohol metabolism in KO and WT mice. However, there were no significant differences in body weight when comparing genotypes at the

Increased alcohol consumption in Urocortin 3 KO

C57Bl/6 mice in this type of paradigm (Crabbe, 1989; Belknap et al., 1993; Yoneyama et al., 2008). Although this fact, in combination with the lack of an obvious effect during peak pain suggests that pain did not influence drinking, the lack of additional control groups remains a limitation of the current study. Additionally, it is still possible that CFA treatment influenced the later drinking patterns of the KO mice specifically, and in the future it will be essential to carefully eliminate the potential effect of chronic pain on drinking behavior in Ucn3 KO mice. Finally, future replication of the increased drinking behavior in Ucn3 KO mice will be beneficial to understanding the current data set. To date, only a limited number of animal studies demonstrate analgesic effects of alcohol (Gatch, 1999, 2006, 2009; Gatch and Lal, 1999). The lack of animal research in this area may be due to the inability to demonstrate alcohol-induced analgesia in a rodent model, as seen with the current results. One consideration is that mice may not be drinking enough to produce antinociception. In fact, previous rodent research showed that while high alcohol doses achieved by an injection or liquid diet can reverse withdrawal induced pain, this does not represent useful analgesia (Gatch, 1999, 2006; Gatch and Lal, 1999). On the contrary, in humans, alcohol can produce analgesia comparable to morphine (James et al., 1978). Another possible explanation for our findings is that alcohol-induced analgesia in rodents is highly sensitive to the timecourse and methodology used. For example, previous research utilizing ethanol-liquid diet exposure in rats found that alcohol-induced analgesia peaked at 2–4 days before dissipating (Gatch, 2009). Therefore the current study may not have captured the peak effect of this phenotype. Alternatively, there may have been development of tolerance to the analgesic effects of alcohol, which has been shown to develop following repeated testing in some cases (Jørgensen and Hole, 1984; Tiffany et al., 1987), but not in others (Gatch and Lal, 1999). Although the current study demonstrates a lack of alcohol analgesia following chronic drinking, we cannot rule out the possibility that alcohol induces analgesia in the rodent following acute intake. It is possible that acute and chronic effects of alcohol produce very different behavioral outcomes. In fact, it is known that abstinence from alcohol following chronic drinking can produce significant changes in pain sensitivity (Katon et al., 1985; Gatch, 2009) and that repeated administration of highly analgesic opiates often leads to hyperalgesia in animals (see Angst and Clark, 2006) and humans (see Lee et al., 2011). Thus, it is possible that the week of alcohol drinking prior to CFA treatment has already led to dysregulation within pain circuitry, and led to an alteration in the response to alcohol. The potential complexity of this phenomenon and the discrepancies in published research solidify the need for meticulous study of the time course and mechanisms of alcohol-induced analgesia. Future research will be essential to understanding the current findings regarding the lack of effect of chronic pain on alcohol drinking, as the current study was lacking certain control groups (water drinking mice and mice that did not receive CFA) necessary to clarify this. In conclusion, we found that genetic deletion of Ucn3 leads to increased intake and preference for alcohol, as expected based on previous research and our theoretical understanding of the involvement of the Urocortin system in alcohol drinking. The direction of this effect is in contrast to that which is observed in male Ucn1 KO mice that can display decreased EtOH preference (Giardino et al., 2011) at lower EtOH

Downloaded from by guest on November 15, 2015

beginning or end of the study (Table 1). Additionally, the gradual development of this phenotype over time argues against any sensitivity differences. Finally, there were no differences between genotypes in BECs on the final day of EtOH access (Fig. 4), despite similar levels of drinking (Fig. 1A and B). This suggests that differences in alcohol metabolism do not explain the current findings. Interestingly, although females drank more than males, there were no significant differences in their blood ethanol levels at the time of sacrifice. This could be explained by the fact that blood was taken 2 h into the dark cycle, before peak drinking levels typically occur. Thus, if we had allowed the animals to drink for at least 4 h into the dark cycle, the pattern of drinking may have led to significantly higher BECs in females when compared with males. Additionally, females drank more but weighed significantly less than male mice (Table 1), therefore we cannot rule out the possibility that alcohol metabolism differs in female and male mice. In fact, previous research has demonstrated that the rate of alcohol disposition in male mice decreases with age compared with females, and this may account for differences in alcohol preference (Collins et al., 1975). As predicted, CFA induced significant and long lasting mechanical pain. However, our studies did not support the role of Ucn3 in chronic inflammatory pain, as there were no differences in the pain behavior of KO and WT mice. This is in contrast to recent findings that Ucn3 may be modulated in certain inflammatory processes (Novembri et al., 2011; Pérez-García et al., 2011), and is located in brain areas relevant to pain modulation (Lewis et al., 2001; Li et al., 2002). Interestingly, basal mechanical thresholds did not significantly change following alcohol access, suggesting a lack of alcohol-induced changes in mechanical sensation. Additionally, we did not find any interactions between pain and drinking. In fact, drinking was only altered following the first day following CFA treatment, and not in the expected direction. Specifically, there was a short-lived decrease in alcohol intake and preference in all mice in the first day of pain. Drinking behavior recovered within 24 h, and did not surpass pre-CFA drinking levels. Thus, the pain state of the mice did not increase alcohol intake or preference at any point in the study, suggesting a lack of alcohol-induced analgesia or alleviation of pain. This observation contrasts with extensive body of literature demonstrating analgesic effects of alcohol in humans (James et al., 1978; Saddler et al., 1985; Woodrow and Eltherington, 1988) and evidence that chronic pain patients often self-medicate with alcohol (Riley and King, 2009). Although the lack of a significant statistical interaction between CFA-induced pain and drinking suggests that presence of pain did not influence alcohol intake, we cannot completely dismiss this possibility. Nevertheless, it seems unlikely that pain is influencing alcohol intake or mediating the differences between the genotypes in the current study for a couple of reasons: (a) The primary differences in drinking between the genotypes do not occur during the peak level of CFAinduced pain, which lasts for 1–2 weeks before recovering (Millan et al., 1988; Chillingworth and Donaldson, 2003; Lu et al., 2008). In fact, in the current study, mechanical thresholds are only statistically decreased from baseline levels during the first two von Frey tests (or the first 14 days) and thus, Ucn3 KO mice appear to be drinking more during or following recovery from pain. (b) The overall level (g/kg) of drinking in the WT mice is similar to that of the typical

137

138

Smith et al.

Funding — This work was supported by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism and the National Institutes of Health (P60 AA01760; U01AA016647; T32AA007468). Conflict of interest statement.

None declared.

REFERENCES Angst MS, Clark JD. (2006) Opioid-induced hyperalgesia: a qualitative systematic review. Anesthes 104:570–87. Belknap JK, Crabbe JC, Young ER. (1993) Voluntary consumption of ethanol in 15 inbred mouse strains. Psychopharmacology 112:503–10. Breese GR, Sinha R, Heilig M. (2011) Chronic alcohol neuroadaptation and stress contribute to susceptibility for alcohol craving and relapse. Pharmacol Ther 129:149–71. Brennan PL, Schutte KK, Moos RH. (2005) Pain and use of alcohol to manage pain: prevalence and 3‐year outcomes among older problem and non‐problem drinkers. Addiction 100:777–86. Chaplan SR, Bach FW, Pogrel JW et al. (1994) Quantitative assessment of tactile allodynia in the rat paw. J Neurosci Meth 53:55–63. Chillingworth NL, Donaldson LF. (2003) Characterisation of a Freund's complete adjuvant-induced model of chronic arthritis in mice. J Neurosci Meth 128:45–52. Collins AC, Yeager TN, Lebsack ME et al. (1975) Variations in alcohol metabolism: influence of sex and age. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 3:973–8. Crabbe JC. (1989) Genetic animal models in the study of alcoholism. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 13:120–7. Deussing JM, Breu J, Kühne C et al. (2010) Urocortin 3 modulates social discrimination abilities via corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor type 2. J Neurosci 30:9103–16. Edwards S, Koob GF. (2010) Neurobiology of dysregulated motivational systems in drug addiction. Future Neurol 5:393–401. Edwards S, Vendruscolo LF, Schlosburg JE et al. (2012) Development of mechanical hypersensitivity in rats during heroin and ethanol

dependence: alleviation by CRF1 receptor antagonism. Neuropharmacology 62:1142–51. Egli M, Koob GF, Edwards S. (2012) Alcohol dependence as a chronic pain disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:2179–92. Fu Y, Neugebauer V. (2008) Differential mechanisms of CRF1 and CRF2 receptor functions in the amygdala in pain-related synaptic facilitation and behavior. J Neurosci 28:3861–76. Funk CK, Koob GF. (2007) A CRF2 agonist administered into the central nucleus of the amygdala decreases ethanol selfadministration in ethanol-dependent rats. Brain Res 1155:172–8. Funk CK, O’Dell LE, Crawford EF et al. (2006) Corticotropinreleasing factor within the central nucleus of the amygdala mediates enhanced ethanol self-administration in withdrawn, ethanol-dependent rats. J Neurosci 26:11324–32. Gatch MB. (1999) Effects of benzodiazepines on acute and chronic ethanol-induced nociception in rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 23:1736–43. Gatch MB. (2006) Tolerance to the antinociceptive effects of ethanol during ethanol withdrawal. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 30:946–52. Gatch MB. (2009) Ethanol withdrawal and hyperalgesia. Curr Drug Abuse Rev 2:41–50. Gatch MB, Lal H. (1999) Effects of ethanol and ethanol withdrawal on nociception in rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 23:328–33. Gianoulakis C, Dai X, Brown T. (2003) Effect of chronic alcohol consumption on the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and pituitary beta-endorphin as a function of alcohol intake, age, and gender. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 27:410–23. Giardino WJ, Ryabinin AE. (2012) Corticotropin-releasing factor: innocent until proven guilty. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:70. Giardino WJ, Cocking DL, Kaur S et al. (2011) Urocortin-1 within the centrally-projecting Edinger-Westphal nucleus is critical for ethanol preference. PLoS One 6:e26997. Goldberg RT, Pachas WN, Keith D. (1999) Relationship between traumatic events in childhood and chronic pain. Disabil Rehabil 21:23–30. Gravanis A, Margioris A. (2005) The corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) family of neuropeptides in inflammation: potential therapeutic applications. Curr Med Chem 12:1503–12. Heilig M, Koob GF. (2007) A key role for corticotropin-releasing factor in alcohol dependence. Trends Neurosci 30:399–406. James M, Duthie AM, Duffy BL. (1978) Analgesic effect of ethyl alcohol. Br J Anaesth 50:139–41. Janssen D, Kozicz T. (2013) Is it really a matter of simple dualism? Corticotropin-releasing factor receptors in body and mental health. Front Endocrinol 4:28. Ji G, Neugebauer V. (2008) Pro- and anti-nociceptive effects of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) in central amygdala neurons are mediated through different receptors. J Neurophysiol 99: 1201–12. Ji G, Fu Y, Ruppert KA et al. (2007) Pain-related anxiety-like behavior requires CRF1 receptors in the amygdala. Mol Pain 3:13. Jørgensen HA, Hole K. (1984) Learned tolerance to ethanol in the spinal cord. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 20:789–92. Katon W, Egan K, Miller D. (1985) Chronic pain: lifetime psychiatric diagnoses. Am J Psychiatry 142:1156–60. Koob GF. (2013) Theoretical frameworks and mechanistic aspects of alcohol addiction: alcohol addiction as a reward deficit disorder. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 13:3–30. Lee M, Silverman SM, Hansen H et al. (2011) A comprehensive review of opioid-induced hyperalgesia. Pain Physician 14:145–61. Lewis K, Li C, Perrin MH et al. (2001) Identification of urocortin III, an additional member of the corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) family with high affinity for the CRF2 receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:7570–5. Li C, Vaughan J, Sawchenko PE et al. (2002) Urocortin III-immunoreactive projections in rat brain: partial overlap with sites of type 2 corticotrophin-releasing factor receptor expression. J Neurosci 22:991–1001. Li C, Chen P, Vaughan J et al. (2007) Urocortin 3 regulates glucose-stimulated insulin secretion and energy homeostasis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:4206–11. Lowery E.G., Spanos M, Navarro M et al. (2010) CRF-1 antagonist and CRF-2 agonist decrease binge-like ethanol drinking in C57BL/

Downloaded from by guest on November 15, 2015

concentrations (6–10%). This fact suggests that there is a complex and orchestrated regulation of EtOH-related behavior by these two neuropeptides (capable of acting on CRFR2 receptors). Whereas Ucn3 is selective for CRFR2, Ucn1 has the capability of acting at CRFR1 and the CRFBP, and this, in combination with some anatomical variation in the expression of these peptides, may account for their unique effects on alcohol drinking. Although alcohol drinking has not been investigated in female Ucn1 KO mice, these mice do display a similar EtOH-induced conditioned place preference as male Ucn1 KO mice (Giardino et al., 2011), which implies that they may also drink less alcohol. Presently, it has not been determined whether Ucn3 KO mice have differences in Ucn1/2 peptide or CRF1/2 receptor expression, yet this could contribute to the current findings. Thus, current research suggests that the Urocortins may play differential roles in alcohol consumption based upon sex and the drinking procedures used, but this needs to be thoroughly investigated in future studies. Finally, although induction of pain with CFA can lead to acute decreases in alcohol drinking, we did not find any longterm effects of chronic pain on alcohol drinking, regardless of genotype. Nor did we find any evidence for alcohol-induced analgesia. These findings highlight the necessity for further characterization of the relationship between alcohol and pain in rodent models in a variety of drinking procedures (e.g.: following other types of pain, or during withdrawal) in order to better understand the overlapping neural mechanisms responsible for these disorders.

Increased alcohol consumption in Urocortin 3 KO 6J mice independent of the HPA axis. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:1241–52. Lu Y, Sun Y-N, Wu X et al. (2008) Role of alpha-amino-3-hydroxy5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionate (AMPA) receptor subunit GluR1 in spinal dorsal horn in inflammatory nociception and neuropathic nociception in rat. Brain Res 1200:19–26. Millan MJ, Członkowski A, Morris B et al. (1988) Inflammation of the hind limb as a model of unilateral, localized pain: influence on multiple opioid systems in the spinal cord of the rat. Pain 35:299–312. McNally GP, Akil H. (2002) Role of corticotropin-releasing hormone in the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in the behavioral, pain modulatory, and endocrine consequences of opiate withdrawal. Neuroscience 112:605–17. Novembri R, Carrarelli P, Toti P et al. (2011) Urocortin 2 and urocortin 3 in endometriosis: evidence for a possible role in inflammatory response. Mol Hum Reprod 17:587–93. Pérez-García S, Juarranz Y, Carrión M et al. (2011) Mapping the CRF-urocortins system in human osteoarthritic and rheumatoid synovial fibroblasts: effect of vasoactive intestinal peptide. J Cell Physiol 226:3261–9. Ren K, Dubner R. (1999) Inflammatory models of pain and hyperalgesia. ILAR J 40:111–8. Reyes TM, Lewis K, Perrin MH et al. (2001) Urocortin II: a member of the corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) neuropeptide family that is selectively bound by type 2 CRF receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:2843–8. Riley JL, III, King C. (2009) Self-report of alcohol use for pain in a multi-ethnic community sample. J Pain 10:944–52.

139

Rouwette T, Vanelderen P, Roubos EW et al. (2012) The amygdala, a relay station for switching on and off pain. Eur J Pain 16:782–92. Ryabinin AE, Tsoory MM, Kozicz T et al. (2012) Urocortins: CRF’s siblings and their potential role in anxiety, depression and alcohol drinking behavior. Alcohol 46:349–57. Saddler JM, James MF, Harington AP. (1985) Naloxone does not reverse ethanol analgesia in man. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 12: 359–64. Sharpe AL, Phillips TJ. (2009) Central urocortin 3 administration decreases limited-access ethanol intake in nondependent mice. Behav Pharmacol 20:346–51. Stewart SH, Finn PR, Pihl RO. (1995) A dose-response study of the effects of alcohol on the perceptions of pain and discomfort due to electric shock in men at high familial-genetic risk for alcoholism. Psychopharmacology 119:261–7. Tiffany ST, McCal KJ, Maude-Griffin PM. (1987) The contribution of classical conditioning to tolerance to the antinociceptive effects of ethanol. Psychopharmacology 92:524–8. Wittmann G, Füzesi T, Liposits Z. (2009) Distribution and axonal projections of neurons coexpressing thyrotropin‐releasing hormone and urocortin 3 in the rat brain. J Comp Neurol 517:825–40. Woodrow KM, Eltherington LG. (1988) Feeling no pain: alcohol as an analgesic. Pain 32:159–63. Yoneyama N, Crabbe JC, Ford MM et al. (2008) Voluntary ethanol consumption in 22 inbred mouse strains. Alcohol 42:149–60. Zorrilla EP, Logrip ML, Koob GF. (2014) Corticotropin releasing factor: a key role in the neurobiology of addiction. Front Neuroendocrinol 35:234–44.

Downloaded from by guest on November 15, 2015

Increased alcohol consumption in urocortin 3 knockout mice is unaffected by chronic inflammatory pain.

Stress neurocircuitry may modulate the relationship between alcohol drinking and chronic pain. The corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) system is cruc...
482KB Sizes 0 Downloads 5 Views