Addictive Behaviors 39 (2014) 558–561

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Addictive Behaviors

Metacognitive mastery moderates the relationship of alexithymia with cluster C personality disorder traits in adults with substance use disorders Paul H. Lysaker a,b,⁎, Kyle Olesek a, Kelly Buck a, Bethany L. Leonhardt c, Jenifer Vohs b, Jamie Ringer a,b, Giancarlo Dimaggio d, Raffaele Popolo d, Jared Outcalt a,b a

Roudebush VA Medical Center, Day Hospital 116H, 1481 West 10th St, Indianapolis, IN 46202, USA Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN 46202, USA c School of Psychological Science, University of Indianapolis, Indianapolis, IN, USA d Center for Metacognitive Interpersonal Therapy, Rome, Italy b

H I G H L I G H T S • • • • •

Cluster C personality disorder traits are common in substance use disorders. Alexithymia may result in the expression of cluster C traits. We tested if metacognition moderated the link of alexithymia with cluster C traits. These constructs were assessed in adults with substance abuse. More cluster C traits were found in persons with both alexithymia and poor mastery.

a r t i c l e

i n f o

Keywords: Substance abuse Personality Alexithymia Metacognition Cluster C personality traits

a b s t r a c t Cluster C personality disorder traits have been observed in substance use disorders and linked with poorer outcome. One potential factor which may cause these disturbances in personality function is alexithymia, or the inability to name and express emotion. There may be other proximate factors which moderate the impact of alexithymia on the expression of cluster C traits, such as metacognitive mastery, which is the ability to use knowledge about mental states of self and others to cope with distress and solve social problems. To examine the possibility that mastery mediated the effects of alexithymia on cluster C traits, we assessed each of these constructs using the Metacognitive Assessment Scale Abbreviated, Toronto Alexithymia Scale and SCID II among 58 adults in an early phase of recovery from substance misuse disorders in a residential setting. Results of a multiple regression revealed that, after controlling for symptom severity and severity of substance misuse history, metacognitive mastery moderated the effect of alexithymia on number of cluster C traits. A median split and subsequent ANCOVA revealed that participants with higher levels of alexithymia and poorer metacognitive mastery had more cluster C traits than the other groups. These findings may have clinical implications, suggesting that patients with substance use disorders may benefit from treatment which addresses metacognitive mastery. Published by Elsevier Ltd.

1. Introduction Cluster C personality disorder traits include dependent, avoidant and obsessive compulsive traits and represent anxious and fearful behaviors which include the seeking of excessive support, control, or avoidance (Cox, Clara, Worobec, & Grant, 2012). They have been noted to co-occur with a range of different mental health conditions (De Rick, Vanheule, & Verhaeghe, 2009; Langås, Malt, & Opjordsmoen, ⁎ Corresponding author at: Roudebush VA Medical Center, Day Hospital 116H, 1481 West 10th St, Indianapolis, IN 46202, USA. E-mail address: [email protected] (P.H. Lysaker). 0306-4603/$ – see front matter. Published by Elsevier Ltd. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.11.007

2012; Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997) and are of interest in the area of substance use disorders given their potential as a barrier to recovery. In order to develop interventions to address cluster C traits, research has sought to understand the factors which promote and sustain these traits. One potential cause of cluster C traits is alexithymia, or difficulties focusing on, identifying and describing feelings (Taylor et al., 1997). Alexithymia has been linked to interpersonal difficulties which closely parallel those seen in cluster C traits (Bekker, Croon, van Balkom, & Vermee, 2008; Joyce, Fujiwara, Cristall, Ruddy & Ogrodniczuk, in press; Taylor et al., 1997; Vanheule, Desmet, Rosseel, Verhaeghe, & Meganck, 2007), and tied to poorer outcomes in substance use disorder including factors which portent risk of relapse (Saladin et al., 2012;

P.H. Lysaker et al. / Addictive Behaviors 39 (2014) 558–561

Thorberg, Young, Sullivan, & Lyvers, 2009; Thorberg et al., 2011; Verrocchio, Conti, & Fulcheri, 2010). While evidence has provided a direct link between alexithymia and cluster C traits in general clinical and substance abuse samples (De-Rick & Vanheule, 2007; Honkalampi, Hintikka, Antikainen, Lehtonen, & Viinama¨ki, 2001; Nicolò et al., 2011) contradictory findings have been reported. Alexithymia has not been linked, for instance, to outcomes in personality disorder (Joyce, Fujiwara, Cristall, Ruddy, & Ogrodniczuk, 2013) or alcohol use disorders (de Haan et al., 2012). Honkalampi et al. (2010) reported that alexithymia was not associated with depression, personality disorder traits, or alcohol use disorders. One possible explanation for these contradictions is that the relationship between alexithymia and cluster C traits in substance use disorders occurs in only some conditions. In other words, the effects of alexithymia on cluster C personality traits may be mediated by other factors. One potential factor that might influence the relationship of alexithymia with cluster C traits is metacognitive dysfunctions. Metacognition refers to a spectrum of activities which involves thinking about thinking and stretches from consideration of discrete psychological phenomenon to the synthesis of discrete perception into an integrated representation of self and others. It has long been considered a problem underlying personality disorders (Dimaggio, Semerari, Carcione, Nicolò, & Procacci, 2007; Dimaggio et al., 2007; Semerari, Carcione, Dimaggio, Nicolò, & Procacci, 2007; Semerari et al., 2003). Metacognition shares aspects in common with the construct of mentalization (Fonagy, Bateman, & Bateman, 2011) though the constructs diverge as mentalization is often considered in the context of attachment, while metacognition may be activated by any of a number of interpersonal motives and includes a specific focus on metacognitive mastery. Metacognitive mastery refers to the use of metacognitive knowledge to respond to psychological and social conflicts (Semerari et al., 2003, 2007). Deficits in metacognitive mastery have been observed in persons with cluster C traits (Carcione et al., 2011; Dimaggio, Procacci, et al., 2007) and linked to outcome in a range of mental disorders including psychosis (Lysaker, Glynn, Wilkness, & Silverstein, 2010; Lysaker et al., 2011) and depression (Ladegaard, Larsen, Videbech, & Lysaker, submitted for publication) as well as medical conditions such as HIV (Ringer et al., submitted for publication). To explore these possibilities, this study assessed alexithymia, metacognitive mastery and cluster C traits among a group of adults with substance use disorders. We predicted that alexithymia would be related to severity of cluster C traits, defined by the number of cluster traits, but that this relationship would be moderated by metacognitive mastery. Specifically, we anticipated that higher levels of cluster C traits would be found with participants with higher levels of alexithymia and lower levels of metacognitive mastery. To rule out the possibility that these findings were the result of heightened levels of psychopathology or severity of lifetime substance abuse we included these as covariates. 2. Method 2.1. Participants Fifty five men and three women with SCID confirmed DSM-IV diagnoses of a substance use disorder were recruited from a residential treatment center associated with VA Medical Center. Seventeen had a primary diagnosis of alcohol dependence, six of alcohol abuse, 31 of polysubstance dependence, two of opiate dependence and two of cocaine dependence. All participants were receiving ongoing treatment and in a post-acute or stable phase of their disorder, defined as no hospitalizations or changes in medication in the last month. All had been abstinent from alcohol and substances for a minimum of one month as confirmed with random drug screens and breathalyzers performed at the residential center. Participants with a SCID confirmed diagnosis of a schizophrenia spectrum disorder were excluded. Participants had a mean age of 43.75 (sd = 10.32), a mean educational level of

559

13.06 years (sd = 1.99) with a mean of 244 (sd = 130.14) months of lifetime drug or alcohol abuse. Forty three participants were Caucasian, 14 African American, and one Latino. 2.2. Instruments 2.2.1. Indiana Psychiatric Illness Interview Indiana Psychiatric Illness Interview (IPII; Lysaker, Clements, Plascak-Hallberg, Knipscheer, & Wright, 2002) is a semi-structured interview typically lasting 30 to 60 min. Responses are audio taped and later transcribed. The interview asks participants to describe and discuss: i) the story of their life in general, ii) whether they think they have a mental illness or substance abuse condition and, if so, how that has affected and not affected their life, iii) how this condition controls and is controlled by them; iv) how it affects, and is affected by others and v) what they see in the future. The IPII differs from other psychiatric interviews in that minimal content is introduced and metacognitive capacities appear spontaneously. 2.2.2. Metacognition Assessment Scale Abbreviated Metacognition Assessment Scale Abbreviated (MAS-A; Semerari et al., 2003) was originally designed to study metacognition within psychotherapy transcripts. It has been abbreviated and adapted for the study of IPII transcripts (Lysaker et al., 2005). For this study we employed the Mastery subscale. Lower Mastery scores reflect difficulties in plausibly describing psychological challenges. Intermediate range scores indicate plausible descriptions of psychological problems with a limited ability to respond to them. Higher scores reflect an ability to respond to psychological challenges effectively on the basis of psychological knowledge. Good interrater reliability was found in this study (intraclass correlation = 0.82). Evidence of the validity of this scale has been presented elsewhere (Lysaker et al., 2011). 2.2.3. The Symptom Checklist-90-R The Symptom Checklist-90-R (SCL-90-R; Derogatis, 1977) is a 90 item self-report inventory designed to assess psychiatric symptom. In this study we used the general psychopathology called the Global Severity Index. 2.2.4. Toronto Alexithymia Scale Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994) is a self-report instrument that assesses awareness of emotional states. For this study we utilized the total score which is a summary of the three subscales. This scale has been used successfully in previous studies of alexithymia (Nicolò et al., 2011). 2.3. Procedure All procedures were approved by the appropriate research review committees. Following informed consent, diagnoses of substance abuse were determined using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID). Participants were then administered the SCID II, TAS, SCL-90 and IPII interview. Mastery was later rated on the basis of a transcription of the IPII using the MAS-A. Table 1 Descriptive statistics of study variables (n = 58). Measures

Mean

sd

Range

SCID II cluster C traits Avoidant traits Dependent traits Obsessive compulsive traits SCID II total traits TAS total MAS-A Mastery SCL-90 GSI T score

4.70 1.48 1.00 2.22 17.56 51.42 3.97 46.43

3.40 1.80 1.62 1.42 11.46 13.81 1.35 10.04

0–14 0–7 0–6 0–5 1–49 23–81 2–7 30–69

560

P.H. Lysaker et al. / Addictive Behaviors 39 (2014) 558–561

Table 2 ANCOVA comparing number of cluster C traits among participants with high and low levels of alexithymia and metacognitive mastery controlling for the SCL-90 Global Severity Index.

Number of cluster C traits M (sd) a

Group 1 High TAS high Mastery (n = 15)

Group 2 High TAS low Mastery (n = 15)

Group 3 Low TAS high Mastery (n = 19)

Group 4 Low TAS low Mastery (n = 9)

TAS effect F(1,54) =

Mastery effect F(1,54) =

Interactiona F(1,54) =

4.53 (3.48)

7.73 (3.17)

3.68 (2.51)

2.11 (1.45)

8.56⁎⁎

0.61

6.57⁎

Fisher's LSD comparisons controlling for SCL-90 GSI T score: Group 2 N groups 1, 3, 4; p b .05; *p b .05; **p b .0001.

3. Results Mean scores for the key variables are presented in Table 1. The modal number of cluster C traits was 2 (n = 12), and the median 3. Twenty-six subjects had five or more cluster C traits. Number of cluster C traits was not significantly correlated with age (r = .05; p = ns), reported lifetime months of drug and alcohol use (r = 0.19, p = ns), education (r = 0.17; p = ns) or MAS-A Mastery scores (r = − 0.16; p = ns). Cluster C traits did not differ between participants with diagnoses of abuse vs. dependence, drug use vs. alcohol use and men vs. women. Number of cluster C traits was significantly related to higher Global Severity Index Scores (GSI; r = 0.46, p b .001) and TAS total scores (r = 0.58; p b .001). The TAS total and Mastery score were inversely related to one another at the trend level (r = −0.25; p = .06). To test the hypothesis that Mastery moderates the relationship of alexithymia with severity of cluster C pathology, independent of general psychopathology, a regression was performed in which the normalized SCL90 GSI score, TAS total and MAS-A Mastery scores were first forced to enter to predict number of cluster C traits and then in a second step the mastery–alexithymia interaction term was allowed to enter. This produced a significant overall model (F(4,53) = 9.85). The R2 for the first step was 0.36 (p b .001) with the interaction raising the total R2 for the model to 0.43 (p b .05). The standardized beta coefficient for the interaction term was −0.27 (p b .05). Given a significant moderation analyses, we next performed a median split and divided participants into high and low groups on the TAS total and MAS-A Mastery scores. Means of these groups are presented in Table 2. As also revealed in Table 2, an ANCOVA comparing groups on number of cluster C traits, controlling for the SCL-90 GSI, revealed a significant group effect for TAS total score and a significant interaction between TAS total score and MAS-A Mastery. Examination of mean scores revealed it was only the group with both higher TAS total scores and lower Mastery scores that had significantly more cluster C traits. Finally, to explore whether there was a relationship between Personality Disorder traits in general, alexithymia and metacognition, we repeated the regression this time predicting total traits on the SCID II. This analysis revealed a significant predictor equation (F(4,53) = 10.92; p b .001) with a significant relationship evidenced between the total traits with the TAS total and the SCL-90 GSI (standardized coefficient beta = 0.38; p b .05; and 0.34; p b .001 respectively). There was however no significant evidence of moderation. 4. Discussion Results suggest that the relationship of alexithymia and severity of cluster C traits in a sample of persons with substance use disorders in early remission was moderated by metacognitive mastery, or the ability to use awareness of mental states to respond to emotional distress and solve interpersonal problems. Specifically, we found that relatively high levels of alexithymia were more closely linked with greater number of cluster C traits only in the presence of deficits in metacognitive mastery after controlling for global psychopathology. Severity of substance use history and type of substance abuse history were unrelated to cluster C traits. While the cross sectional nature of the data precludes drawing causal conclusions, findings may suggest some hypotheses for future

research. For one, it is possible that being unable to name one's own feelings does not necessarily develop into fearful or anxious patterns of interpersonal relationships in substance use disorders on its own, but that it must only occur in the absence of the ability to use knowledge about oneself and others. Importantly, there are limitations. Participants were mostly men in their 40s involved in treatment. More research is needed with younger persons, females, or among persons who decline treatment. Severity of substance misuse was calculated in terms of lifetime months of abuse and forms of substance use and thus nuances may have been lost. More research is also necessary which involves collecting a broader array of data at multiple time points. With replication, our findings may have several clinical implications. In particular it may be useful to consider interventions that target metacognition. One promising avenue of therapy now being developed is Metacognitive Interpersonal Therapy (Dimaggio, Attinà, Popolo, & Salvatore, 2012) which targets cluster c problems. Persons with cooccurring substance abuse and cluster C traits may benefit from this kind of treatment which is aimed at helping them use metacognitive knowledge to take a critical distance from their negative expectations about others and balance them with views of themselves as realistically effective, able and deserving of appreciation. Role of funding source This study was funded by the Indiana Institute for Medical Research. This body played no role in study design; the collection, analysis and interpretation of data, in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Contributors Lysaker and Outcalt designed the study. Lysaker performed all data analysis. Lysaker, Olesek, Buck, Leonhardt, Vohs, Ringer, Dimaggio, Popolo and Outcalt contributed to the literature review and the manuscript preparation including interpretation of findings.

Conflict of interest There are no conflicts of interest or disclosures.

References Bagby, R. M., Parker, J.D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-I. Item selection and cross validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 23–32. Bekker, M. H., Croon, M.A., van Balkom, E. G., & Vermee, J. B. (2008). Predicting individual differences in autonomy-connectedness: The role of body awareness, alexithymia, and assertiveness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64(6), 747–765. Carcione, A., Semerari, A., Nicolò, G., Pedone, R., Popolo, R., Conti, L., et al. (2011). Metacognitive mastery dysfunctions in personality disorder psychotherapy. Psychiatry Research, 190, 60–71. Cox, B. J., Clara, I. P., Worobec, L. M., & Grant, B. F. (2012). An empirical evaluation of the structure of DSM-IV personality disorders in a nationally representative sample: Results of confirmatory factor analysis in the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions waves 1 and 2. Journal of Personality Disorders, 26(6), 890–901. de Haan, H. A., Schellekens, A. F., van der Palen, J., Verkes, R. J., Buitelaar, J. K., & De Jong, C. A. (2012). The level of alexithymia in alcohol-dependent patients does not influence outcomes after inpatient treatment. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 38(4), 299–304. De Rick, A., & Vanheule, S. (2007). Attachment styles in alcoholic inpatients. European Addiction Research, 13(2), 101–108. De Rick, A., Vanheule, S., & Verhaeghe, P. (2009). Alcohol addiction and the attachment system: An empirical study of attachment style, alexithymia, and psychiatric disorders in alcoholic inpatients. Substance Use & Misuse, 44, 99–114. Derogatis, L. R. (1977). SCL-90-R, administration, scoring & procedures manual-I for the revised version. : Johns Hopkins School of Medicine.

P.H. Lysaker et al. / Addictive Behaviors 39 (2014) 558–561 Dimaggio, G., Procacci, M., Nicolò, G., Popolo, R., Semerari, A., Carcione, A., et al. (2007). Poor metacognition in narcissistic and avoidant personality disorders: Analysis of four psychotherapy patients. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 14, 386–401. Dimaggio, G., Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Nicolò, G., & Procacci, M. (2007). Psychotherapy of personality disorders: Metacognition, states of mind and interpersonal cycles. London: Routledge. Fonagy, P., Bateman, A., & Bateman, A. (2011). The widening scope of mentalization: A discussion. Psychology and Psychotherapy, 84, 98–110. Honkalampi, K., Hintikka, J., Antikainen, R., Lehtonen, J., & Viinama¨ki, H. (2001). Alexithymia in patients with major depressive disorder and comorbid cluster c personality disorders: A 6-month follow-up study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 3, 245–254. Honkalampi, K., Koivumaa-Honkanen, H., Lehto, S. M., Hintikka, J., Haatainen, K., Rissanen, T., et al. (2010). Is alexithymia a risk factor for major depression, personality disorder, or alcohol use disorders? A prospective population-based study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 68(3), 269–273. Joyce, A. S., Fujiwara, E., Cristall, M., Ruddy, C., & Ogrodniczuk, J. S. (2013). Clinical correlates of alexithymia among patients with personality disorder. Psychother Research, 23(6), 690–704. Ladegaard, N., Larsen, E. R., Videbech, P., & Lysaker, P. H. (submitted for publication). Higher-order social cognition in first-episode major depression. Psychiatry Research (submitted for publication). Langås, A.M., Malt, U. F., & Opjordsmoen, S. (2012). In-depth study of personality disorders in first-admission patients with substance use disorders. BMC Psychiatry, 12, 180. Lysaker, P. H., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Johannesen, J. K., Nicolò, G., Procacci, M., et al. (2005). Metacognition amidst narratives of self and illness in schizophrenia: Associations with insight, neurocognition, symptom and function. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 112, 64–71. Lysaker, P. H., Clements, C. A., Plascak-Hallberg, C. D., Knipscheer, S. J., & Wright, D. E. (2002). Insight and personal narratives of illness in schizophrenia. Psychiatry, 65, 197–206. Lysaker, P. H., Erickson, M.A., Ringer, J., Buck, K. D., Semerari, A., Caricione, A., et al. (2011). Metacognition in schizophrenia: The relationship of mastery to coping, insight,

561

self-esteem, social anxiety and various facets of neurocognition. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50(4), 412–424. Lysaker, P. H., Glynn, S. M., Wilkness, S. M., & Silverstein, S. M. (2010). Psychotherapy and recovery from schizophrenia: A review of potential application and need for future study. Psychological Services, 7(2), 75–91. Nicolò, G., Semerari, A., Lysaker, P. H., Dimaggio, G., Conti, L., D'Angerio, S., et al. (2011). Alexithymia in personality disorders: Correlations with symptoms and interpersonal functioning. Psychiatry Research, 190(1), 37–42. Ringer, J. M., Outcalt, S., Buck, K. D., Leonhardt, B., Vohs, J., & Lysaker, P. H. (submitted for publication). Mastery as a predictor of pain in patients with HIV. (submitted for publication). Saladin, M. E., Santa Ana, E. J., LaRowe, S. D., Simpson, A. N., Tolliver, B. K., Price, K. L., et al. (2012). Does alexithymia explain variation in cue-elicited craving reported by methamphetamine-dependent individuals? American Journal on Addictions, 21(2), 130–135. Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Falcone, M., Nicolo, G., Procacci, M., et al. (2003). How to evaluate metacognitive function in psychotherapy? The Metacognition Assessment Scale its applications. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 10, 238–261. Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Nicolò, G., & Procacci, M. (2007). Understanding minds, different functions and different disorders? The contribution of psychotherapeutic research. Psychotherapy Research, 17, 106–119. Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Parker, J.D. A. (1997). Disorders of affect regulation. Alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Thorberg, F. A., Young, R. M., Sullivan, K. A., & Lyvers, M. (2009). Alexithymia and alcohol use disorders: A critical review. Addictive Behaviors, 34(3), 237–245. Thorberg, F. A., Young, R. M., Sullivan, K. A., Lyvers, M., Connor, J. P., & Feeney, G. F. (2011). Alexithymia, craving and attachment in a heavy drinking population. Addictive Behaviors, 36(4), 427–430. Vanheule, S., Desmet, M., Rosseel, Y., Verhaeghe, P., & Meganck, R. (2007). Relationship patterns in alexithymia: A study using the Core Conflictual Relationship Theme (CCRT) method. Psychopathology, 40, 14–21. Verrocchio, M. C., Conti, C., & Fulcheri, M. (2010). Deliberate self-harm in substance-dependent patients and relationship with alexithymia and personality disorders: A case–control study. Journal of Biological Regulators and Homeostatic Agents, 24(4), 461–469.

Metacognitive mastery moderates the relationship of alexithymia with cluster C personality disorder traits in adults with substance use disorders.

Cluster C personality disorder traits have been observed in substance use disorders and linked with poorer outcome. One potential factor which may cau...
195KB Sizes 0 Downloads 0 Views