©Journal of Sports Science and Medicine (2014) 13, 767-773 http://www.jssm.org

Research article

Motor Learning as Young Gymnast’s Talent Indicator Alessandra di Cagno 1 , Claudia Battaglia 1, Giovanni Fiorilli 2, Marina Piazza 3, Arrigo Giombini 2, Federica Fagnani 1, Paolo Borrione 1, Giuseppe Calcagno 2 and Fabio Pigozzi 1 1

Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Italian University of Sport and Movement of Rome “Foro Italico”, Rome, Italy; 2 Department of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Molise, Campobasso, Italy; 3 Department of Anatomy, Histology and Forensic Medicine, University of Florence, Florence, Italy

Abstract Talent identification plans are designed to select young athletes with the ability to achieve future success in sports. The aim of the study was to verify the predictive value of coordination and precision in skill acquisition during motor learning, as indicators of talent. One hundred gymnasts, both cadets (aged 11.5 ± 0.5 yr.) and juniors (aged 13.3 ± 0.5 years), competing at the national level, were enrolled in the study. The assessment of motor coordination involved three tests of the validated Hirtz’s battery (1985), and motor skill learning involved four technical tests, specific of rhythmic gymnastics. All the tests were correlated with ranking and performance scores reached by each gymnast in the 2011, 2012, and 2013 National Championships. Coordination tests were significantly correlated to 2013 Championships scores (p < 0.01) and ranking (p < 0.05) of elite cadet athletes. Precision, in skill acquisition test results, was positively and significantly associated with scores in 2013 (adj. R2 = 0.26, p < 0.01). Gymnasts with the best results in coordination and motor learning tests went on to achieve better competition results in three- year time. Key words: Training, precision, coordination, talent selection, gymnastics, motor skills.

Introduction One of the main tasks of National Sports Federations is to identify, select, and prepare athletes for international competitions such as the Olympics Games, World and European Championships, and World Cups. Sport managers and coaches try to pick out future champions using psychological, physical, technical, and anthropometric criteria (Harris and Atkinson, 2009). However, if the selection process is attempted too early, it could lead to numerous mistakes, and it is possible for a promising champion to fail the enrollment at a relatively early age. A correct selection is fundamental because, after some years of deliberate practice, the gap between selected and unselected athletes becomes insurmountable, and it is impossible to dredge talented athletes who were previously excluded by mistake (Bullock et al., 2009). When considering anthropometric measurements and motor skills as good indicators to select athletes (di Cagno et al., 2008), it is necessary to keep in mind that during puberty, which is an unstable period, the evaluation of young athletes is complicated by individual differences in the timing of changes in body size and functional capacities (Philippaerts, 2006; Unnithan et al., 2013). It is possible that pre-adolescents, who possess the characteris-

tics required by a sport, will not retain these qualities throughout maturation until adulthood and that these innate capacities may not turn into exceptional adult sport performance (Ackland and Bloomfield, 1996; Beunen et al., 1981). On the contrary, many of the qualities that distinguish the performance of an excellent adult athlete do not necessarily appear evident during childhood or puberty (Philippaerts, 2006). To avoid prematurely eliminating talented young athletes who are currently not “performing”, it is essential to distinguish between variables that influence performance and those that influence development (Abbot and Collins, 2002). Biological maturation affects morphology and fitness more so than motor coordination skills (Vaeyens et al., 2006). Recent findings showed moderate to high long-term stability in coordinative skills and sportspecific skills from childhood to adolescence (Ahnert et al., 2009; Vaeyens et al., 2006). A motor coordination test might be valuable in the early talent identification of gymnasts, as its discriminative and predictive qualities might be sufficiently powerful for selection (Vandorpe et al., 2012). Motor coordination has a high stability as talent indicator for child to adolescent athlete selection (Ahnert et al, 2009). Motor learning capacity may be considered a prerequisite for the learning of specific skills in gymnastics and is especially strong in the pre-adolescent period, identified as a "sensitive phase" in which the talent selection is carried out (Hirtz and Starosta, 2002; Starosta and Hirtz, 1989), recognizing and exploiting the capacity to develop. It is essential to select based on an athlete’s ability to develop, rather than simply on a good performance level at the time of testing (Abbott and Collins, 2002; 2004). Motor learning capacity may be a determinant of the potential that an individual has to develop within a sport. The Italian Gymnastics Federation has used two traditional strategies in current talent selection programs, consisting of the competition results evaluation and specific rhythmic gymnastics tests. Both of these are strongly affected by the gymnast’s personal experience in their clubs. The main purpose of the study was to verify whether the predictive value of coordination and precision in skill acquisition during motor learning, could be valid and long-term indicators in selecting talent among young rhythmic gymnasts, and related to performance in competition three years later. The precision in the tests requires body segments to be held correctly during the movements, as well as good balance, good amplitude of

Received: 15 January 2014 / Accepted: 26 June 2014 / Published (online): 01 December 2014

768

Talent selection in gymnastics

Table 1. Technical element descriptions at each level. Leap Pivot Risk with throw 1 Rope: 720° Front split One rotation around the horizontal Entrelacé Cadets “with help” axis of the body with passing to the floor and catch without hands Ball: Entrelacé 360° (or more) One rotation around the horizontal with split axis of the body with passing to the Junior Splits “without leap and with floor and catch out of the visual help” ring field and without hands

movement shapes, and precise direction and plane of execution. We hypothesized that motor coordination and skill acquisition tests could be valuable in the early identification of gymnasts, both in cadet and in junior gymnastics categories, and has a predictive value for future performances in competition, independently from the gymnast’s level of performance (elite or sub-elite).

Methods Participants One hundred athletes of rhythmic gymnastics were enrolled in the study including 41 elite competing at the national level, divided into two age categories: 20 cadets (age: 11.5 ± 0.5; training: 25.0 ± 10.2 hours per week-1) and 21 juniors (age: 13.3 ± 0.5; training: 30.7 ± 14.1 hours per week-1) and 59 sub-elite competing at the regional level (age: 10.5 ± 0.5; training: 20.5 ± 3.3 hours per week-1). The Italian Gymnastics Federation selected the athletes, elite and sub-elite, to participate in different national summer trainings, depending on technical levels. The participants were selected for the 2010 national summer training. The following exclusion criteria were applied: less than two years of gymnastic training and no health problems or injuries. The study was designed according to the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local Ethics Committee. Parents of underage athletes (less than 18 years old) gave their written consent for the study. Study design Three non-sport specific motor coordination tests within the battery validated by Hirtz (1985) and four technical elements specific of rhythmic gymnastics, had been correlated with ranking and performance scores reached by each gymnast in the 2011, 2012, and 2013 National Championships, to verify if coordination and precision in skill acquisition during motor learning, could be valid and long-term indicators in selecting talent among young rhythmic gymnasts. Technical elements and assessments The four specific technical elements were a leap, a specific tour named “pivot”, and two "risk with throw of apparatus" elements. Table 1 provides descriptions of the four technical elements and Figure 1 provides schematics of the four elements at each level. The selected elements required high coordinative skills and were unknown to the gymnasts. The chosen jump required lower limb kinesthetic discrimination and orientation ability (Pehoiu, 2010). The chosen pivot required a high level of dynamic

Risk with throw 2 Hoop: One rotation around the horizontal axis of the body with passing to the floor and re-throw of the apparatus Clubs: One rotation around the vertical axis of the body without passing to the floor and re-throw of the apparatus and during the flight one rotation around the horizontal axis of the body

balance and limb response orientation ability. The two "risk with throw" elements required upper limb kinesthetic discrimination abilities and dynamic balance. The three motor coordination tests assessed kinesthetic discrimination, response orientation abilities, and dynamic balance. The “low jump test” assessed lower limb kinesthetic discrimination ability. The “orientation shuttle run test” assessed lower limb response orientation ability. The “backwards ball throw test” assessed upper limb kinesthetic discrimination ability. Table 2 provides descriptions of the three motor coordination tests. This battery has high test/retest reliability (Intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC)) (r = 0.97, p = 0.000) (Zetou, et al., 2012). The evaluation was based on precision in the execution of the tests, following the penalties of the International Code of Points in Rhythmic Gymnastics (ICP) (International Rhythmic Gymnastics Committee, 2013). The precision in the tests requires body segments to be held correctly during movements as well as good balance, good amplitude of movement shapes, and precise direction and plane of execution. Procedures In the 2010 national summer training the gymnasts underwent a pre-test based on three motor coordination tests and four specific technical element tests after a 30-minute warm up. Each element was first explained verbally by the coaches and then demonstrated by an athlete. An evaluation scale from zero to five points, with penalties of 0.10 for small faults, 0.20 for medium faults and 0.30 to 0.50 for major faults, based on ICP criteria, was chosen for this study. Each element was repeated three times and the best results were considered. In the training intervention the gymnasts were allowed ten days to learn the technical elements. Training sessions were twice per day (20 sessions), half an hour for each session. Three sets of ten trials for each element, with 30-sec. rest periods between trials and two-min. rest periods between sets were performed in each session. Each set was followed by coach feedback. The evaluation of the specific technical elements was carried out in the middle of the experimental period (intermediate-test scores on the fifth day) and at the end (post-test scores on the 10th day). The ranking and the scores of the 2011, 2012, and 2013 National Championships in which the gymnasts had participated were collected. Statistical analysis The sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test for normality was

di Cagno et al.

769

applied to check that sample data were normally distributed. Pearson product moment correlation was conducted to assess the relationship between the variables measured at the initial testing and the 2011, 2012, and 2013 competition outcomes. The amount of variance in ranking and scores in

the three National Championships was calculated using linear regression analysis for each group separately. The α level was set at p ≤ 0.05, and the analyses were conducted using SPSS statistical package software (SPSS, v.20.0 Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Jump “entrallace” for cadets

Jump “entrallace with ring” for juniors

“Pivot 720° front split with help” for cadets

“Pivot 360° splits without help” for juniors

Risk 1 with rope for cadets

Risk 1 with ball for juniors

Risk 2 with hoop for cadets

Risk 2 with clubs for juniors Figure 1. Schematics for the technical elements.

770

Talent selection in gymnastics

Table 3. Pearson’s correlations between Hirtz’s battery tests and the score and ranking in the 2011, 2012 and 2013 National Championships. Junior Cadet elite Cadet sub elite Score Ranking Score Ranking Score Ranking 2011 2012 2013 2011 2012 2013 2011 2012 2013 2011 2012 2013 2011 2012 2013 2011 2012 2013 Low jump (cm) .29 .06 -.35 .18 -.08 .36 -.23 -.11 -.54** .46* .26 .65** -.44** -.35* -.28* .30* .26* .38** -.28 .47* -.55** .04 .19 .42 -.06 -.21 -.27 .04 .03 .13 -.20 -.17 -.27 .28* .27* .27* OSRT (sec) .41 -.13 .40 -.15 -.37 -.25 .28 .23 .59** -.13 -.12 -.50* .02 -.09 .03** .09 .09 -.03 BBTT (score) OSRT: Orientation shuttle run test . BBTT: Backwards ball throw test ; * Significant at 0.05 level, ** Significant at 0.01 level

Table 2. Hirtz’s battery test descriptions. Test Description To assess lower limb kinesthetic discrimination ability. Participants jumped with the legs together from a plinth to a ground marking at a set distance (1 m). They were instructed to land with their heels on the Low jump test marking. The test was performed twice and the distance of each heel from the marking was measured in centimeters for each trial. Distance values were collapsed across heels and trials to obtain one mean value. To assess lower limb response orientation ability. The participant was instructed to run three times, as quickly as possible, from a start marker toward one of five numbered goal markers located behind her. The goal markers were 3m apart from her and 1.5m apart from another on a hypoOrientation thetical circumference arc. The sequence of shuttle goal markings to be reached was not run test known previously. The next marking number was announced when the participant returned to the start ball and touched it for the next run to begin without pausing. After demonstration by an experimenter, participants performed the test that was scored in seconds. To assess upper limb kinesthetic discrimination ability. Participants performed a one-hand overhead throw backwards with a tennis ball. They were instructed to center a ground target located 250 cm behind the performer. The target had a 20 cm diameBackwards ter. After a training throw, participants ball throw test performed five consecutive trials. Five points were assigned for each centered target. Scores of 4, 3, 2, 1 and 0 were assigned with increasing distance of the contact point of the ball from the target and the mean score was computed.

Results The P value of the sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test results (p = 0.05 in leap; p = 0.060 in pivot; p = 0.120 in ‘risk with throw 1’; p = 0.971 in ‘risk with throw 2’) confirmed that the sample data was normally distributed. Correlation coefficients between Hirtz’s battery tests and score and ranking in the 2011, 2012, and 2013 National Championships are shown in Table 3. Significant correlation between motor learning precision and both ranking and scores in 2011, 2012, and 2013 National Championships are shown in Table 4. By linear regression analyses we examined the predictive value of those variables that significantly corre-

lated with the results in competition. Considering junior athlete, the “Orientation shuttle run test” score predicted the results in competition three years later and explained a significant proportion of variance in competition scores (R2 = 0.30; adj R2 = 0.26; p < 0.05). Considering cadet elite, low jump test and backwards ball throw test explained the variance in 2013 competition scores (R2 = 0.28; adj R2 = 0.24; p < 0.05 and R2 = 0.35; adj R2 = 0.31; p < 0.01, respectively). For cadet sub elite low jump test explained the variance in 2013 competition scores (R2 = 0.08; adj R2 = 0.06; p < 0.05). The linear regression analyses showed that precision in motor skills learning explained the variance in the scores of elite (2011: adj R2 = 0.05, p < 0.05; 2012: adj R2 = 0.119 p < 0.01) and sub elite cadet gymnasts (2011: adj R2 = 0.225, p < 0.01; 2012: adj R2 = 0.176 p < 0.01). Precision explained the variance of elite cadet scores in 2013 (adj R2 = 0.489, p < 0.01) (Figure 2). Table 4. Pearson’s correlations (r) between motor learning precision with the scores and ranks gained in the 2011, 2012, 2013 National Championships. Pearson’s Correlation National Championship Scores Rank 2011 .238 * -.278 ** 2012 .359 ** -.217 * 2013 .522 ** -.444 ** * Significant at 0.05 level ** Significant at 0.01 level

Discussion The aim of the present longitudinal study was to verify the predictive value of coordination and precision in skill acquisition during motor learning, as indicators of talent, in order to foresee future competition outcomes for young gymnasts. The main result was that coordination and precision in the motor learning process could be longterm predictors of a gymnast’s potential for achieving future success in competition and consequently a more sensitive measurement of ability in the sport at an early age (Vaeyens et al., 2006). Precision in skill acquisition explained 50% of the variance three years later in competition scores for elite cadets, whereas only 20% in competition scores for sub elite cadets and juniors, affirming that precision in the motor learning process is highly predictive for young promising gymnasts. Cadet athletes who are at a golden age for motor learning (Starosta and Hirtz, 2002), characterized by sensitive phases for coordinative skills (Hirtz and Starosta, 2002; Starosta and Hirtz, 1989), showed better motor learning ability for new technical elements than juniors. Juniors who demonstrated current ability rather than the potential to excel (Philippaerts et al., 2008)

di Cagno et al.

771

were more constant performers but were not precise with new movements (Biro et al., 2003). A clear stagnation and impairment could be expected during pubertal growth (Starosta and Hirtz, 1989). Given that this trend is more evident in elite gymnasts, it follows that motor learning may be a better indicator of talent for elite athletes than sub-elite.

Junior

Cadet Elite

Cadet Sub Elite Figure 2. Linear regression analyses using motor skill learning precision as predictor of scores and ranking of 2013 Italian National Championship.

A novelty of this study was the proposal of specific gymnastic elements to test the gymnast’s motor learning ability and to predict future results for top-level athletes. The authors, to better measure the learning progression, utilized specific tests, which were able to distinguish

between elite and sub-elite athletes (BenOunis et al., 2013) and the potential of talented gymnasts to develop. Even if the general coordinative traits should be assessed with non-sport specific tests, which are useful to select lower level athletes (Vandorpe et al., 2012), an high correlation between lower and upper limb kinesthetic discrimination, and cadets 2011, 2012, 2013 scores and ranking was found. Orientation shuttle run test significantly correlated to 2012 and 2013 junior championship scores, highlighting that this test is based not only on coordination, but also strength and rapidity, showed only by junior gymnasts. Precision in the learning process, evaluated by specific tests, predicts talent outcomes better than coordination ability assessed with standardized and validated tests, because rhythmic gymnastics bases gymnastic preparation on the repetition of technical elements until perfect execution is attained. The variability in coordination tests found in this study highlighted that Hirtz’s battery (1985) do not seem to be sensitive enough to discriminate gymnasts at the top level. The precision in skill acquisition characterizes expert performance in almost all sport activities (Abbot and Collins, 2002; Ericsson et al., 1993) and plays an essential role in achieving relevant results in rhythmic gymnastics (Kioumourtzoglou et al., 1997; Miletic et al., 2004). The ability for new motor element acquisition is minimally impacted by training, age, and maturation. A high motor learning and coordination ability in new element acquisition were not significantly affected by a gymnast’s background experience (such as technical coaching and increased competitive standard), which could lead to an uncorrected talent selection. Vandorpe’s (2012) and Abernethy’s (2008) research results supported the present findings. When considering 2011 and 2012 championships, precision showed low predictive value on gymnast scores especially for elite and sub elite cadets. The higher correlation between motor learning assessment and competition results in 2013 rather than in 2012 and 2011 indicated that the predictive power of this indicator grows over time, underlining the potential attainment of long-term expertise. This selection procedure answers to a dynamic conception of talent detection in which learning and adaptation should be viewed as the extreme characteristics of a talented athlete (Ericsson et al., 1993). Trainability and skill learning will allow the acceleration of talent development through additional competition and training opportunities (Vaeyens et al., 2006). Considering the specific tests one by one, the best technical improvement was found in the most complex tests, such as pivot and ‘elements at risk’. These technical elements require greater coordination ability, dynamic balance, multi-limb combination, and orientation than others such as jumps. Jump realization requires improvement in several muscular abilities such as strength and stiffness, which are predictive talent indicators (di Cagno, et al., 2008), but not developed at this age (Weineck, 2009).

Conclusion

772

In talent identification and selection procedures it is better to include the evaluation of coordination and motor learning ability. Motor learning assessment concerns performance improvement and the ability to develop it, rather than evaluating the athlete’s current performance. In this manner talent identification processes should be focused on the future performance capabilities of athletes. The authors presented pilot data to introduce a more holistic and specific long-term method able to identify and select talented gymnasts with predictive competitive success over a period of three years. The present study underlined the advantage of using specific gymnastics tests rather than validated non-specific test batteries. In the field of practice, it could be useful for coaches to include motor learning tests in selection procedures, as objective criteria that could discriminate between elite and sub-elite pre-adolescent gymnasts. Motor learning tests should be added in the first talent selection procedure as maturity independent tests (Vandendriessche et al., 2012), complementing the coach’s intuitive observations and judgments (BenOunis et al., 2013). Acknowledgements This study was not supported by grants. Authors state that there are no professional relationships with companies or manufacturers who will benefit from the results of the present study.

References Abbott, A. and Collins, D.A. (2002) Theoretical and empirical analysis of state of “a state of the art” talent identification model. High Ability Studies 13, 157-178. Abernethy, B. (2008) Introduction: Developing expertise in sport – how research can inform practice. In: Developing Sport Expertise: Researchers and Coaches Put Theory into Practice. Eds: Farrow, D., Baker, J. MacMahon, C. Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge. 1-14. Ackland, T.R. and Bloomfield, J. (1996) Stability of human proportions through adolescent growth. Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 28, 57-60. Ahnert, J., Shneider, W. and Bos, K. (2009) Developmental changes and individual stability of motor abilities from the preschool period to young adulthood. In: Human development from early childhood to early adulthood. Findings from a 20 year longitudinal study. Eds: Shneider, W., Bullock, M., Mahwah, N.J. New York: Psychology Press. 35-62. BenOunis, O., BenAbderrahman, A., Chamari, K., Ajmol, A., Benbrahim, M., Hammouda, A., BenBrahim, M., Hammami, M. and Hassane, Z. (2013) Association of short-passing ability with athletic performances in youth soccer players. Asian Journal of Sports Medicine 4, 41-48. Beunen, G., Ostyn, M., Simons, J., Renson, R. and Van Gerven, D. (1981) Chronological and biological age as related to physical fitness in boys 12 to 19 years. Annals of Human Biology 8, 321331. Biro, F.M., Lucky, A.W., Barton, B.A., Daniels, S.R., Striegel-Moore, R., Kronsberg, S.S. and Morrison, J.A. (2003) Pubertal maturation in girls and the relationship to anthropometric changes: pathways through puberty. Journal of Pediatrics 142, 643-646. Bullock, N., Gublin, J.P., Martin, D.T., Ross, A., Holland, T. and Marino, F. (2009) Talent identification and deliberate programming in skeleton: ice novice to Winter Olympian in 14 months. Journal of Sport Science 27, 397-404. Di Cagno, A., Baldari, C., Battaglia, C., Brasili, P., Merni, F., Piazza, M. and Guidetti, L. (2008) Leaping ability and body composition in rhythmic gymnasts for talent identification. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness 48, 341-346. Di Cagno, A., Baldari, C., Battaglia, C., Guidetti, L. and Piazza, M. (2008) Anthropometric characteristics evolution in elite rhythmic gymnasts. Italian Journal of Anatomy and Embryology 113, 29-35.

Talent selection in gymnastics

Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T. and Tesch-Romer, C. (1993) The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review 100, 363-406. Harris, D.J. and Atkinson, G. (2009) International Journal of Sports Medicine – Ethical standards in Sport and Exercise Science Research. International Journal of Sports Medicine 30, 701-702. Hirtz, P. and Starosta, W. (2002) Sensitive and critical periods of motor co-ordination development and its relation to motor learning. Journal of Human Kinetics 7, 19-28. Hirtz, P. Arndt, H.J., Holtz, D., Jung, R., Ludwig, G., Schielke, E., Wellnitz, I., Willert H.J. and Vilkner, H.J. (1985) Coordinative abilities in physical education. Volk and Wissen Verlag, Berlin. International Rhythmic Gymnastics Committee. (2013) Code of Point. Edition 2013-2016. Kioumourtzoglou, E., Derri, V., Mertzanidou, O. and Tzetzis, G. (1997) Experience with perceptual and motor skills in rhythmic gymnastics. Perceptual & Motor Skills 84, 1363-1372. Miletić, Đ., Katić, R. and Maleš, B. (2004) Some anthropologic factors of performance in rhythmic gymnastics novices. Collegium Antropologicum 26, 533-538. Pehoiu, C. (2010) Spatio-temporal orientation development during the physical education class, with 5th and 6th form pupils. World Academy of Science, Engineering and Technology, 72. Philippaerts, R.M. (2006) The relationship between peak height velocity and physical performance in youth soccer. Journal of Sports Science 24, 221-230. Philippaerts, R.M., Coutts, A. and Vaeyens, R. (2008) Physiological perspectives on the identification and development of talented performers in sport. In: Talent identification and development. The source for sporting excellence. Eds: Fisher, R and Bailey, R. eds. Berlin: ICSSPE. 49-67. Starosta, W. and Hirtz, P. (1989) Sensitive and critical periods in development of coordination abilities in children and youths. Biology of Sport 6, 276-282. Unnithan, V., White, J., Georgiou, A., Iga, J. and Drust, B. (2013) Talent identification in youth soccer. Journal of Sports Sciences 30, 1719-1726. Vaeyens, R., Gullich, A., Chelsea, R.W. and Philippaerts, R.M. (2009) Talent identification and promotion programmes of Olympic athletes. Journal of Sports Sciences 27, 1367-1380. Vaeyens, R., Malina, R.M., Janssens, M., Van Renterghem, B., Bourgois, J., Vrijens, J. and Philippaerts, R.M. (2006) A multidisciplinary selection model for youth soccer: The Ghent Youth Soccer Project. British Journal of Sports Medicine 40, 928-934. Vandendriessche, J.B., Vaeyens, R., Vandorpe, B., Lenoir, M., Lefevre, J. and Philippaerts, R.M. (2012) Biological maturation, morphology, fitness, and motor coordination as part of a selection strategy in the search for international youth soccer players (age 15-16 years). Journal of Sports Sciences 30, 1695-1703. Vandorpe, B., Vandendriessche, J.B., Vaeyens, R., Pion, J., Lefevre, J., Philippaerts, R.M. and Lenoir M. (2012) The value of a nonsport-specific motor test battery in predicting performance in young female gymnasts. Journal of Sports Science 30, 497-505. Weineck, J. (2009) L’allenamento Ottimale. Calzetti Mariucci, Perugia. Zetou, E., Vernadakis, N., Tsetseli M., Kampas, A., and Michalopoulou, M. (2012) The effect of coordination training program on learning tennis skills. The Sport Journal. Available form URL: http://thesportjournal.org/article/the-effect-of-coordinationtraining-program-on-learning-tennis-skills/

Key points • In talent identification and selection procedures it is better to include the evaluation of coordination and motor learning ability. • Motor learning assessment concerns performance improvement and the ability to develop it, rather than evaluating the athlete’s current performance. • In this manner talent identification processes should be focused on the future performance capabilities of athletes.

di Cagno et al.

773

AUTHORS BIOGRAPHY Alessandra di CAGNO Employment Ass. Prof., Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Italian University of Sport and Movement of Rome “Foro Italico”, Rome, Italy Degree PhD Research interests Sport Sciences and Coaching E-mail: [email protected] Claudia BATTAGLIA Employment University tutor with part-time contract, Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Italian University of Sport and Movement of Rome “Foro Italico”, Rome, Italy Degree PhD Research interests Sport Sciences and Coaching E-mail: [email protected] Giovanni FIORILLI Employment Manager, Department of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Molise, Campobasso, Italy Degree PhD Research interests Sport Sciences and Coaching E-mail: [email protected] Marina PIAZZA Employment Assoc. Prof., Italian Gymnastics Federation Director, Department of Anatomy, Histology and Forensic Medicine, University of Florence, Florence, Italy Degree PhD Research interests Sport Sciences and Coaching E-mail: [email protected] Arrigo GIOMBINI Employment Ass. Prof., Department of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Molise, Campobasso, Italy Degree MD Research interests Sport Medicine E-mail: [email protected] Federica FAGNANI Employment Ass. Prof., Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Italian University of Sport and Movement of Rome “Foro Italico”, Rome, Italy Degree MD Research interests Sport Medicine E-mail: [email protected]

Paolo BORRIONE Employment Ass. Prof., Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Italian University of Sport and Movement of Rome “Foro Italico”, Rome, Italy Degree MD Research interests Sport Medicine E-mail: [email protected] Giuseppe CALCAGNO Employment Ass. Prof., Department of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Molise, Campobasso, Italy Degree MD Research interests Sport Medicine E-mail: [email protected] Fabio PIGOZZI Employment Full Associated Professor, Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Italian University of Sport and Movement of Rome “Foro Italico”, Rome, Italy Degree MD Research interests Sport Medicine E-mail: [email protected]

Dott. Alessandra di Cagno PhD Department of Movement, Human and Health Science, University of Rome “Foro Italico”, 00198, Italy

Motor Learning as Young Gymnast's Talent Indicator.

Talent identification plans are designed to select young athletes with the ability to achieve future success in sports. The aim of the study was to ve...
668KB Sizes 1 Downloads 9 Views