The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse Encompassing All Addictive Disorders

ISSN: 0095-2990 (Print) 1097-9891 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/iada20

Neurocognitive and psychiatric dimensions of hot, but not cool, impulsivity predict HIV sexual risk behaviors among drug users in protracted abstinence Michael J. Wilson PhD & Jasmin Vassileva PhD To cite this article: Michael J. Wilson PhD & Jasmin Vassileva PhD (2016) Neurocognitive and psychiatric dimensions of hot, but not cool, impulsivity predict HIV sexual risk behaviors among drug users in protracted abstinence, The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 42:2, 231-241, DOI: 10.3109/00952990.2015.1121269 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00952990.2015.1121269

Published online: 02 Feb 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 72

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 1 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=iada20 Download by: [Cornell University Library]

Date: 04 October 2016, At: 02:02

AM J DRUG ALCOHOL ABUSE 2016, VOL. 42, NO. 2, 231–241 http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00952990.2015.1121269

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Neurocognitive and psychiatric dimensions of hot, but not cool, impulsivity predict HIV sexual risk behaviors among drug users in protracted abstinence Michael J. Wilson, PhDa and Jasmin Vassileva, PhDb a

Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA; bDepartment of Psychiatry, Institute for Drug and Alcohol Studies, Virginia Commonwealth University School of Medicine, Richmond, VA, USA ABSTRACT

ARTICLE HISTORY

Background: Impulsivity is an important risk factor for HIV risky drug and sexual behaviors. Research identifies hot (i.e. affectively-mediated, reward-based) and cool (motoric, attentional, independent of context) neurocognitive and psychiatric dimensions of impulsivity, though the impact of specific drugs of abuse on these varieties of impulsivity remains an open question. Objectives: The present study examined the associations of neurocognitive and psychiatric varieties of hot and cool impulsivity with measures of lifetime and recent sexual risk behaviors among users of different classes of drugs. Methods: The study sample was comprised of drug users in protracted (> 1 year) abstinence: heroin mono-dependent (n = 61), amphetamine mono-dependent (n = 44), and polysubstance dependent (n = 73). Hot impulsivity was operationalized via neurocognitive tasks of reward-based decision-making and symptoms of psychopathy. Cool impulsivity was operationalized via neurocognitive tasks of response inhibition and symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Results: Hot impulsivity was associated with sexual risk behaviors among heroin and amphetamine users in protracted abstinence, whereas cool impulsivity was not associated with sexual risk behaviors among any drug-using group. Neurocognitive hot impulsivity was associated with recent (past 30-day) sexual risk behaviors, whereas psychopathy was associated with sexual risk behaviors during more remote time-periods (past 6 month and lifetime) and mediated the association between heroin dependence and past 6-month sexual risk behaviors. Conclusion: Assessments and interventions aimed at reducing sexual risk behaviors among drug users should focus on hot neurocognitive and psychiatric dimensions of impulsivity, such as decision-making and psychopathy. Cool dimensions of impulsivity such as response inhibition and ADHD were not related to sexual risk behaviors among drug users in protracted abstinence.

Received 21 April 2015 Revised 9 November 2015 Accepted 13 November 2015

Introduction New cases of HIV infection continue to appear at an alarming rate worldwide, due in large part to high rates of sexual risk behaviors among at-risk populations (1,2). Resources for HIV treatment are scarce (3) and better characterization of factors influencing HIV risk behavior is needed to inform prevention efforts. Impulsivity, the “predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internal or external stimuli without regards to the negative consequences of these reactions” (4) is implicated in a wide variety of sexual risk behaviors. Impulsivity is a multidimensional construct with state-like neurocognitive manifestations, as well as more pervasive psychiatric manifestations in externalizing syndromes like attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and psychopathy. These psychiatric syndromes are characterized by developmental manifestations of impulsivity that predict risk behaviors across the lifespan (5–7), whereas

KEYWORDS

Impulsivity; substance dependence; protracted abstinence; HIV risk behavior; externalizing psychopathology; decision-making

neurocognitive functioning is more state-dependent and may be more sensitive to imminent risk behavior. Drug users often display psychiatric (8–10) and neurocognitive manifestations of impulsivity (11–14). The unique pharmacological properties of different classes of drugs may lead to differential expressions of impulsivity among users of different types of drugs (15–18); e.g. stimulant users demonstrate greater executive deficits (19–21) and higher levels of risk behavior (17,22–24) than opiate users, whereas opiate users are more impaired on tasks of decision-making and feedback learning (25–27). Two broad neurocognitive dimensions of impulsivity have been identified that are mediated by dissociable neurobiological substrates: hot impulsivity, an affectively-mediated preference for immediate gratification in the presence of anticipatory cues (28,29); and cool impulsivity, an affectively neutral tendency towards rapid, premature responses, regardless of context (14,30,31). These two dimensions are

CONTACT Jasmin Vassileva, PhD [email protected] Department of Psychiatry, Institute for Drug and Alcohol Studies, Virginia Commonwealth University School of Medicine, 203 E. Cary Street, Richmond, VA 23219, USA. © 2016 Taylor & Francis

232

M. J. WILSON & J. VASSILEVA

distinct in factor analyses (32–34) and are discriminately associated with different forms of externalizing psychopathology and related neural substrates. Specifically, hot impulsivity is associated with psychopathy and ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction (35–37) while cool impulsivity is associated with ADHD and inferior fronto-striato-cerebellar systems (34,37–39). Few studies have examined neurocognitive risk factors for HIV risk behavior. Available research indicates that premorbid global cognitive function (40) and decision-making (41–44) are associated with HIV risk behavior in HIV-seropositive drug users. Research focusing on psychiatric risk factors reveal associations with externalizing psychopathology: psychopathy is as a robust predictor of sexual risk behaviors (45,46), while ADHD correlates with sexual risk behaviors independently of antisocial traits (47). The extent to which externalizing psychopathology and neurocognitive impulsivity differ in their contributions to HIV risk behavior among drug users is not well understood. Furthermore, high rates of polysubstance dependence among research samples (25,48,49) limit inferences regarding specific effects of different drug classes on impulsivity (50,51). Conducting research in Eastern Europe circumvents this methodological difficulty; despite high rates of drug use, mono-dependence is more common among Eastern European than Western drug users due to the relatively recent influx of black market heroin and proliferation of synthetic drug laboratories in Eastern Europe (52). Although preliminary research has linked neurocognitive impulsivity to sexual risk behaviors among recently active drug users (41–44), no studies have examined such risk factors among drug users in protracted abstinence – a critical gap in the literature given evidence of persisting addiction-induced neuroadaptations and high risk for relapse and associated risk behaviors in protracted abstinence (53–55). To address these gaps, the present study examined how sexual risk behaviors were influenced by neurocognitive and psychiatric indices of hot and cool impulsivity. Study participants were Bulgarian monodependent heroin users, mono-dependent amphetamine users, and polysubstance users in protracted abstinence. Due to the protracted abstinence of the current sample, injection drug use data was not examined. We hypothesized that lifetime dependence on any class of drugs would show positive associations with sexual risk behaviors, as would both neurocognitive and psychiatric measures of hot and cool impulsivity.

Keeping with the putative state-trait distinction (i.e. neurocognitive functioning represents more statedependent impulsivity, whereas psychiatric symptoms represent developmentally stable manifestations of trait impulsivity), we further hypothesized that neurocognitive impulsivity would be selectively associated with recent sexual risk behaviors, while psychiatric impulsivity was hypothesized to demonstrate selective associations with more distal sexual risk behaviors over the lifespan. Selective associations of hot versus cool psychiatric dimensions of impulsivity were not predicted due to conflicting findings in the literature and a paucity of research examining these factors in relation to sexual risk behaviors.

Methods Participants Participants were recruited for a larger study of impulsivity among stimulant and heroin users, advertised via flyers at addiction clinics and public locales in Sofia, Bulgaria. Participants were screened via telephone or on-site and provided informed consent. All drug users met criteria for lifetime dependence on heroin or amphetamines. Community adults with similar demographics who had no history of substance dependence were recruited as controls. Length of abstinence was indexed via self-report during clinical interviews. The majority of drug users reported being in protracted abstinence (i.e. DSM-IV “full sustained remission” for more than one year; see Table 1) at the time of testing, and only drug users in protracted abstinence were included in analyses. Inclusion criteria were: (a) age 18–50 years; (b) 8th grade education; (c) IQ > 75; (d) no history of neurologic illness/serious TBI; (e) no evidence of psychotic/thought disorders; (f) negative alcohol Breathalyzer screen; and (g) negative urine screen for opiates, cannabis, amphetamines, methamphetamines, benzodiazepines, barbiturates, cocaine, MDMA, and methadone. All participants were HIVseronegative (determined by rapid HIV testing) and none were on opioid substitution therapy. The final sample (N = 281) consisted of 103 controls, 44 amphetamine users, 61 heroin users, and 73 polysubstance users (see Table 1). Procedures All self-report and interview measures were translated into Bulgarian by the senior author, a native Bulgarian speaker and author of the Bulgarian translation of a

THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF DRUG AND ALCOHOL ABUSE

233

Table 1. Participant group characteristics. Controls (n = 103) Age (M, SD) Education (M, SD) Ravens IQ (M, SD) Sex (# men, %)

24.5a 13.6a 108.6a 78

Amphetamine (n = 44)

(4.8) (2.4) (14.0) [76]

DSM-IV lifetime substance dependence (#, %) Amphetamine 0 Heroin 0 Alcohol 0 Cannabis 0 Cocaine 0 Hallucinogens 0 Sedatives 0 Days since last met dependence criteria (Mdn, IQR) Amphetamine — Heroin — Sexual risk behaviors (M, SD) RAB Past 6 month HRBS Past 30 day HRBS Lifetime SRB Knowledge

2.6a 3.2 8.6a 11.7

(1.7) (2.8) (3.9) (2.1)

Externalizing psychopathology (M, SD) PCL:SV 4.8a (3.9) WURS 21.5a (12.1) Neurocognitive impulsivity composites (Z-scores; M, SD) Hot impulsivity −0.2 (0.9) n = 72 Cool impulsivity −0.3 (1.9) n = 77

22.5b 12.5b 109.6a 31

Heroin (n = 61)

26.8d 12.9b 106.0a,b 64

44 [100] 0 0 0 0 0 0

0 61 [100] 0 0 0 0 0

45 38 24 56 5 5 9

639 {1075} —

— 730 {1035}

3.1b 3.0 10.6b 10.9

(1.6) (2.5) (3.8) (2.2)

9.0b (4.4) 28.3b (13.4) 0.1 (0.92) n = 31 0.3 (1.9) n = 39

29.1c 12.8b 103.1b 47

Polysubstance (n = 73)

(4.5) (2.4) (13.5) [77]

(3.3) (1.8) (11.9) [70]

3.5b 4.1 12.4c 11.9

(1.9) (2.7) (4.0) (2.1)

12.7c (4.8) 31.2b (15.8) 0.2 (1.0) n = 54 0.3 (1.6) n = 54

F = 20.6** F = 3.1* F = 3.0* χ2 = 5.8

[62] [52] [33] [77] [7] [7] [12]

OR = 1.6** OR = 1.9** — — — — —

639 {1095} 545 {588} 3.2b 9.8 12.6c 11.7

Significance testing

(5.3) (2.2) (12.8) [88]

(1.9) (3.7) (4.0) (2.2)

12.3c (4.8) 33.3b (16.6) −0.1 (0.9) n = 51 0.2 (2.2) n = 55

U = 228 U = 360 F = 4.3** F = 2.0 F = 18.5** F = 2.3 F = 60.0** F = 11.3** F = 1.8 F = 1.6

Identical superscripts indicate that group values do not differ statistically; *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; RAB, Risk Assessment Battery; HRBS, HIV Risk Behavior Scale; SRB, Sexual Risk Behavior; PCL:SV, Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version; WURS, Wender Utah Rating Scale.

widely used English-language assessment instrument. All measures were back-translated into English by Bulgarian psychiatrists and psychologists. The study protocol consisted of two 3.5-hour sessions, completed at the Bulgarian Addictions Institute. The first session was devoted to assessment of addictive disorders, externalizing psychopathology, and intelligence. The second session included neurocognitive testing and selfreport assessments of externalizing and internalizing personality traits and psychopathology. Participants were paid 80 Bulgarian Leva (≈ US$50) for participation. Procedures were approved by Institutional Review Boards of the University of Illinois at Chicago and the Medical University in Sofia on behalf of the Bulgarian Addictions Institute. Measures IQ was estimated via Raven’s Progressive Matrices (57). History of DSM-IV substance dependence and duration of abstinence from substance dependence criteria (Table 1) were obtained via a Bulgarian translation of the substance abuse module from the SCID-I (58). HIV sexual risk behavior Practical knowledge about safe sexual practices and HIV risk reduction strategies was assessed by a

15-item self-report HIV knowledge scale (56). HIV risk behavior was indexed via the HIV Risk Behavior Scale (HRBS; 59) and the Risk Assessment Battery (RAB; 60). The RAB is a 24-item interviewer-completed scale assessing frequency and quantity of injection drug use (IDU) and sexual risk behaviors within the past six months. The HRBS is an 11-item interviewer-completed scale assessing IDU and sexual risk behaviors. We administered both the past 30-day sexual risk behaviors and lifetime sexual risk behaviors versions of the HRBS. We used both the HRBS and the RAB in order to capture three relatively distinct temporal phases of risk behavior: (a) acute risk behavior (i.e. HRBS past 30-day); (b) recent risk behavior (i.e. RAB past 6 months); and (c) lifetime risk behavior (HRBS). Given that all drug user groups reported virtually no injection drug use over the past 30 days (F(3,276) = 0.96, p = 0.49) or past 6 months (F(2,175) = 0.38, p = 0.68) and that amphetamine users reported virtually no lifetime injection drug use relative to controls (t(145) = 0.10, p = 0.85), IDU data was not analyzed. Psychiatric measures of impulsivity Psychiatric dimensions of hot impulsivity were operationalized by symptoms of psychopathy and were indexed using the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL:SV; [61]), a 12-item interviewer-completed scale

234

M. J. WILSON & J. VASSILEVA

based on a semi-structured interview. Interviews and psychopathy ratings were conducted by research assistants who were trained to reliability and supervised closely by the senior author, who has extensive clinical experience with psychopathy. A recent construct validation of the Bulgarian PCL:SV (62) supported an oblique two-factor structure, with Factor 1 (F1) reflecting affective and interpersonal aspects of psychopathy and Factor 2 (F2) encapsulating poor behavioral control and antisociality. Full-scale psychopathy scores were used as predictor variables. Psychiatric dimensions of cool impulsivity were operationalized by childhood symptoms of ADHD and assessed via the 25-item version of the self-report Wender Utah Rating Scale (WURS; 63). Respondents were asked to retrospectively evaluate the presence and severity of childhood symptoms of ADHD (sample item: “As a child I had concentration problems, was easily distracted”). Total WURS scores were employed as predictor variables. Neurocognitive measures of hot impulsivity To assess neurocognitive hot impulsivity, participants completed two decision-making tasks: the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT; 28) and the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT; 64); and the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (65), a measure of delay discounting. The IGT measures decision-making under uncertainty and requires trial-and-error learning. Examinees are presented with four decks of cards and instructed to select cards to maximize earnings. Decks A and B are associated with higher rewards but also higher occasional penalties. Selecting from Decks C and D yields lower rewards and lower occasional penalties and is a more advantageous long-term strategy. The performance measure used was total number of advantageous choices minus total number of disadvantageous choices. The CGT assesses risky decision-making, which does not involve implicit learning. Examinees are presented with 10 boxes colored red or blue and are asked to guess whether a token is hidden under a red or a blue box. The ratios of red:blue boxes vary from 1:9 to 9:1 in pseudorandom order. Participants earn points based on correct performance. They also gamble points based on the confidence of their decisions, by selecting from an array of bets ranging from 5–95% of their earned points, presented in ascending and descending order. Three performance measures were selected based on their established sensitivity to risky decision-making among individuals with addictive disorders (66–70): (a) quality of decision-making, the tendency to bet on the more likely outcome; (b) risk adjustment, betting more when odds are better and less when odds are

poorer; and (c) delay aversion, or betting larger amounts earlier when wagers are presented in ascending order. The Monetary Choice Questionnaire consists of 27 choices between smaller rewards available on the day of testing and larger rewards available from 1 week to 6 months in the future and captures the tendency to discount the value of delayed rewards. Analyses utilized the discount-rate parameter, k, determined using the hyperbolic discount function V = A/[1 + kD], where V is the value of reward A available at delay D. Neurocognitive measures of cool impulsivity Neurocognitive cool impulsivity was assessed using the Go/No-Go Task (GNGT; 71); the Immediate Memory Task (IMT; 30); and the Go-Stop Task (GST; 72). The GNGT is a measure of response inhibition where a series of two-element visual stimuli arrays are presented on a screen for 500 ms and examinees are instructed to respond when the two elements are identical (“Go”) and to inhibit responding when the stimuli are discrepant (“No-Go”). On “No-Go” trials, the position of the inhibitory element is random, requiring the examinee to scan both elements. The performance measure, d’, indexes the ability to discriminate between “Go” and “No-Go” stimuli. The IMT is a modified continuous performance test of higher complexity and sensitivity. A series of fivedigit numbers are shown for 500 ms each, with examinees instructed to respond only if a stimulus is identical to the preceding display. The performance index, d’, measures the ability to discriminate between targets and non-targets. The GST (70) is a stop-signal paradigm, which presents examinees with a series of five-digit numbers displayed for 500 ms each. Examinees are instructed to respond when a stimulus is identical to the previous display (“Go”) and to withhold responding when the stimulus matches, but then changes color from black to red (“Stop”). Stop-signals occurred at 50, 150, 250, or 350 ms intervals from stimulus onset. The performance measure was the ratio of inhibition failures on “Stop trials” to correct responses on “Go” trials across the four stop-signal delays. Statistical plan Variable skew and kurtosis were inspected for normality. Neurocognitive variables were converted to Z-scores based on total sample means and collapsed into two composites based on a priori theoretical distinctions between hot and cool impulsivity from the literature

THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF DRUG AND ALCOHOL ABUSE

(32–34,37). The hot impulsivity composite included the IGT score, the three CGT parameters, and the delay discounting parameter k. The cool impulsivity composite included the discriminability parameters (d’) from the IMT and the GNGT and the mean inhibition ratio from the GST. To maintain consistency with psychiatric dimensions, the directionality of certain Z-scores was reversed for specific neurocognitive tasks (i.e. IGT; CGT decision-making/risk adjustment; all cool neurocognitive tasks) so that higher composite scores indicated more impulsive performance. A composite score was not computed for participants with missing data on any task forming that composite. One-way ANOVAs, Mann-Whitney U tests, and Chi-square tests were used to probe for group differences on demographic measures with follow-up Fisher’s LSD and Exact tests were conducted when indicated. Hierarchical multiple linear regressions examined effects of drug type, impulsivity dimensions, and their interactions on sexual risk behaviors. Separate models were calculated for hot and cool impulsivity. Age, education, and gender were entered in Block 1 as control variables. Three dummy-coded drug user groups (heroin, amphetamine, polysubstance) were entered in Block 2, with control participants serving as references. The neurocognitive composite of either hot impulsivity and psychopathy or cool impulsivity and ADHD was entered in Block 3. The analyses were structured in this fashion to control for the effects of previous substance dependence (Block 2) before entering impulsivity variables (Block 3), providing a relatively conservative test for determining the significance of impulsivity. Additionally, observing a change in significance from Block 2 to Block 3 would serve to indicate whether any observed effects of impulsivity

235

represented incremental validity beyond effects of substance dependence. Finally, interactions of drug user groups with hot or cool impulsivity were entered in Block 4. See Table 2 for a correlation matrix of all variables used in analyses.

Results Participant characteristics Participant demographics are presented in Table 1. Group differences (ps < 0.05) were observed for age, education, and estimated IQ. Knowledge of safe sexual practices was equivalent across groups (ps > 0.05), suggesting that differences in sexual risk behaviors could not be attributed to lack of knowledge about HIV and risk reduction strategies. All groups reported equivalent past 30-day sexual risk behaviors. Heroin and polysubstance users reported greater levels of past 6-months sexual risk behaviors than controls (ps < 0.05), with a trend-level difference observed between amphetamine users and controls (p = 0.06). All drug user groups demonstrated elevated levels of lifetime sexual risk behaviors, with heroin and polysubstance users reporting higher levels of lifetime sexual risk behaviors than amphetamine users (ps < 0.05). Drug users evidenced more psychopathic traits than controls, with heroin and polysubstance users exhibiting higher psychopathy than amphetamine users (ps < 0.05). All drug users reported equivalent levels of ADHD symptoms (ps > 0.10) that were elevated versus controls (ps < 0.05). There were no group differences on tasks of neurocognitive hot or cool impulsivity (ps > 0.05).

Table 2. Correlation matrix of variables used for analyses. Demographic variables Age Demographics Age Sex Edu

Sex

– −0.21** −0.21** – 0.20** 0.15*

Sexual risk behaviors RAB 6mo 0.17** 0.02 HRBS 30d 0.11 0.01 HRBS Lifetime 0.31** −0.14*

Edu 0.20** 0.15* –

Sexual risk behaviors RAB 6mo HRBS 30d HRBS Lifetime 0.17** 0.02 0.08

0.11 0.01 −0.004

Externalizing psychopathology

Neurocognitive impulsivity composites

PCL:SV

WURS

Hot

Cool

0.31** −0.14* 0.01

0.22** −0.31** −0.21**

0.13* −0.11 −0.11

−0.10 0.01 −0.27

−0.13* 0.12 −0.17* 0.01 0.08 −0.04

– 0.45** 0.41**

0.45** – 0.23*

0.41** 0.23** –

0.23** 0.16** 0.43**

0.18** 0.04 0.18**

0.13 0.17* −0.03

Externalizing psychopathology PCL:SV 0.22** −0.31** −0.21** WURS 0.13* −0.11 −0.11

0.23** 0.18**

0.16** 0.04

0.43** 0.18**

– 0.43**

0.43** –

0.18** −0.02

0.14* 0.004

Neurocognitive impulsivity composites Hot −0.10 0.01 −0.27** Cool −0.13* 0.12 −0.17*

0.13 0.01

0.17* 0.08

0.18** 0.14*

0.03 0.000

– 0.25**

0.25** –

0.08 −0.004 0.01

−0.03 −0.04

*p ≤ 0.05; **p < 0.01; Edu, Years of Education; Sex coded 1, male, 2, female; RAB, Risk Assessment Battery; 6 mo, past 6 months; HRBS, HIV Risk Behavior Scale; 30 d, past 30 days; PCL:SV, Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version; WURS, Wender Utah Rating Scale.

236

M. J. WILSON & J. VASSILEVA

Effects of hot impulsivity on sexual risk behaviors Results of all hot impulsivity regressions are presented in Table 3. Neither demographics (Block 1 ΔR2 = 0.02, p = 0.41) nor drug user type (Block 2 ΔR2 = 0.04, p = 0.096) explained significant variance in past 30-day sexual risk behaviors. Block 3 explained significant variance (ΔR2 = 0.04, p = 0.01) and demonstrated that neurocognitive hot impulsivity tasks explained unique variance in past 30-day sexual risk behaviors (β = 0.16, p = 0.03). Block 4 explained additional unique variance in past 30-day sexual risk behaviors (ΔR2 = 0.08, p = 0.002). Polysubstance × psychopathy (β = −0.48, p = 0.04) and polysubstance × neurocognitive hot impulsivity (β = −0.18, p = 0.05) interactions were observed, with simple slope analyses indicating that neither psychopathy symptoms nor neurocognitive hot impulsivity task performance were associated with past 30-day sexual risk behaviors among polysubstance users (ps > 0.10). Demographic variables did not account for variance in past 6-month sexual risk behaviors (Block 1 ΔR2 = 0.04, p = 0.11). Block 2 explained unique variance in past 6-month sexual risk behaviors (ΔR2 = 0.04, p = 0.03), driven by a main effect of heroin dependence (β = 0.25, p = 0.006). Additional unique variance was explained in Block 3 (ΔR2 = 0.07, p < 0.001) due to a main effect of psychopathy (β = 0.32, p < 0.001). Block 4 explained further unique variance (ΔR2 = 0.06, p < 0.001), driven by a significant polysubstance × psychopathy interaction; when followed up, this interaction indicated that psychopathy was not associated with past 6-month sexual risk behaviors among polysubstance users (p = 0.003, follow-up p = 0.10).

Variance in lifetime sexual risk behaviors was associated with substance dependence across all drug user types in Block 2 (ΔR2 = 0.13, p < 0.001): amphetamine (β = 0.29, p < 0.001); heroin (β = 0.35, p < 0.001); polysubstance (β = 0.31, p < 0.001). Block 3 explained additional unique variance in lifetime sexual risk behaviors (ΔR2 = 0.06, p < 0.001) due to a main effect of psychopathy (β = 0.32, p < 0.001). Block 4 accounted for additional unique variance (ΔR2 = 0.05, p = 0.03) due to amphetamine × psychopathy and polysubstance × psychopathy interactions (ps < 0.04); Follow-up testing indicated that psychopathy and lifetime sexual risk behaviors were uncorrelated among amphetamine and polysubstance users (ps > 0.10). Effects of cool impulsivity on sexual risk behaviors Main effects of ADHD symptoms and neurocognitive cool impulsivity on measures of sexual risk behavior were non-significant (ps > 0.15); see Table 4. Although a polysubstance × cool impulsivity interaction was noted for past 30-day sexual risk behaviors, this effect was not investigated further due to the lack of significant variance explained by the regression model (Block 4 ΔR2 = 0.02, p = 0.63). Post-hoc analyses Correlations between past-30 day sexual risk behaviors and individual hot neurocognitive measures were conducted to elucidate which specific neurocognitive processes appeared to drive the observed effect in regression models. Results indicated a significant, negative association of IGT (r = −0.14, p = 0.02) and a

Table 3. Hierarchical multiple linear regressions examining the effects of drug user type, psychopathic personality features, and hot neurocognitive impulsivity on sexual risk behavior. Past 30-day sexual risk behaviors ΔR2 Step 1: Control variables Step 2: Drug user groups Amphetamine Heroin Polysubstance Step 3: Main effects of psychopathy and hot impulsivity composite PCL:SV Hot impulsivity Step 4: Drug user group interactions with psychopathy and Hot impulsivity composite Amphetamine × PCL:SV Amphetamine × Hot impulsivity Heroin × PCL:SV Heroin × Hot” impulsivity Polysubstance × PCL:SV Polysubstance × Hot” impulsivity

β

0.02 0.04

Past 6-month sexual risk behaviors ΔR2

0.08 0.22* 0.02 0.04*

0.07**

0.08**

β

0.29** 0.36** 0.31** 0.06**

0.32** 0.07 0.06*

−0.31 0.04 −0.01 −0.13 −0.48* −0.18*

ΔR2 0.16** 0.13**

0.12 0.25** 0.14

0.12 0.16*

*p ≤ 0.05; **p < 0.01; PCL:SV, Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version.

β

0.04 0.04*

Lifetime sexual risk behaviors

0.32** −0.06 0.05*

−0.32 −0.002 −0.39 −0.06 −0.67** −0.08

−0.34* 0.02 −0.33 −0.05 −0.61** −0.04

THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF DRUG AND ALCOHOL ABUSE

237

Table 4. Hierarchical multiple linear regressions examining the effects of drug user type, ADHD symptoms, and cool neurocognitive impulsivity on sexual risk behavior. Past 30-day sexual risk behaviors Past 6-month sexual risk behaviors Lifetime sexual risk behaviors Step 1: Control variables Step 2: Drug user groups Amphetamine Heroin Polysubstance Step 3: Main effects of ADHD and cool impulsivity WURS Cool impulsivity Step 4: Drug user group interactions with ADHD and Cool impulsivity composite Amphetamine × WURS Amphetamine × Cool impulsivity Heroin × WURS Heroin × Cool impulsivity Polysubstance × WURS Polysubstance × Cool impulsivity

ΔR2 0.02 0.01

β

ΔR2 0.03 0.03

0.05 0.14 0.06 0.01

−0.01 0.08

0.03

β

ΔR2 0.14** 0.11**

0.12 0.17 0.20* 0.01

0.25** 0.31** 0.34** 0.01

0.06 0.05 0.05

−0.14 0.04 −0.18 -0.02 −0.43 −0.09*

β

0.09 −0.04 0.02

0.07 0.15 −0.22 0.08 −0.39 −0.01

-0.04 0.10 0.24 −0.08 0.002 −0.01

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; WURS, Wender Utah Rating Scale total score.

marginally significant, negative correlation of CGT quality of decision-making (r = −0.12, p = 0.05), indicating that common hot decision-making processes under both conditions of ambiguity (i.e. IGT) and explicit risk (i.e. CGT) contributed to the observed effect on past 30-day sexual risk behavior. By contrast, risk adjustment and delay aversion on the CGT and delay discounting were not associated with past-30 day sexual risk behavior (ps ≥ 0.12). Correlations between F1 and F2 of the PCL:SV and past 6-month and lifetime SRB were conducted to determine whether different factors of psychopathy appeared to drive the observed effects in regression models. Both factors of psychopathy were significantly associated with risk measures (past 6-month F1 r = 0.22, p < 0.001; F2 r = 0.22, p < 0.001; lifetime F1 r = 0.31, p < 0.001; F2 r = 0.37, p < 0.001). The main effect of heroin dependence on past 6month sexual risk behaviors observed in Block 2 became non-significant (β = 0.04, p = 0.87) after adding Block 3 containing psychopathy, indicating that psychopathy may partially mediate the relationship between heroin dependence and past 6-month sexual risk behaviors. A post-hoc test of mediation was computed using the PROCESS macro for SPSS (73). Results indicated significant effects of heroin dependence on psychopathy (β = 1.78, p = 0.006); an effect of psychopathy on sexual risk behaviors (β = 0.08, p < 0.001); and a direct effect of heroin dependence on sexual risk behaviors (β = 0.47, p = 0.04). Finally, a reduced indirect effect (β = 0.15, 95% CI 0.046–0.305) of heroin dependence on sexual risk behaviors was observed when controlling for psychopathy, indicating a partial mediating effect of psychopathy.

Discussion To our knowledge, the present study is the first to demonstrate that neurocognitive and psychiatric dimensions of hot impulsivity, but not cool impulsivity, selectively predict sexual risk behaviors among drug users in protracted abstinence. Additionally, we believe that our findings are the first to indicate that neurocognitive hot impulsivity shows selective associations with recent (i.e. past 30-day) sexual risk behaviors across drug users in protracted abstinence, whereas a measure of externalizing psychopathology characterized by hot impulsivity (i.e. psychopathy) had differential associations with sexual risk behaviors across the lifespan. Finally, our findings demonstrated that psychopathy mediates associations of prior heroin dependence and sexual risk behaviors during the withdrawal and early abstinence periods (i.e. past 6-months) of the addiction cycle. Effects of hot neurocognitive impulsivity and psychopathy on sexual risk behaviors were observed after controlling for history of drug dependence, suggesting that both neurocognitive and psychiatric indices of affective impulsivity provide incremental utility towards predicting sexual risk behaviors across different stages of the addiction cycle. Neurocognitive hot impulsivity was selectively associated with past-30 day sexual risk behaviors across both amphetamine and heroin users in protracted abstinence, similar to associations between hot neurocognitive impulsivity and sexual risk behaviors observed among HIV-seropositive and seronegative drug users with more recent substance use (41–44). Additionally, the association of psychopathy with sexual risk behaviors among heroin users in protracted abstinence is similar to previous findings in active methadone

238

M. J. WILSON & J. VASSILEVA

maintenance patients (46,74–76) suggesting that the link between psychiatric hot impulsivity and sexual risk behaviors persists in protracted abstinence. This is supported by the mediating effect of psychopathy on the association between history of heroin dependence and past 6-month sexual risk behaviors observed in the present sample. In contrast, psychopathy was not associated with sexual risk behaviors among polysubstance users in protracted abstinence after controlling for history of substance dependence. This result may be influenced by confounding of psychopathy and the tendency to misuse multiple drugs (77), such that controlling for polysubstance dependence may also parcel out variance associated with psychopathy. Among amphetamine users in protracted abstinence, psychopathy was associated with past 6-month sexual risk behaviors, but this relationship was not observed for lifetime sexual risk behaviors. Speculatively, relatively acute effects of amphetamine dependence may have been associated with sexual risk behaviors during periods of active use, whereas psychopathy may have emerged as a more pronounced risk factor during a more recent (i.e. past year) period of abstinence from amphetamines. Impaired performance on neurocognitive tasks of impulsivity has been consistently documented among individuals in the acute and early abstinence stages of addiction (11,78–81). By contrast, the current sample of drug users in protracted abstinence was unimpaired on composite indices of impulsivity tasks. However, previous analyses have indicated significant group differences on individual neurocognitive measures in this population (51,82) and computational modeling has revealed that distinct neurocognitive processes underlie hot decision-making performance in heroin versus amphetamine users in protracted abstinence (82).

Conclusions

Limitations

Declaration of interest

The present cross-sectional study has several limitations. Although statistically controlled for, systematic demographic differences may have influenced results. Secondly, the WURS depends on retrospective recall of ADHD symptoms in childhood. Third, psychopathy’s relationship to sexual risk behaviors may be explained only in part by impulsivity, as evidenced by significant correlations between both F1 and F2 with sexual risk behaviors. Fourth, cultural differences may influence results, although cross-cultural validity has been established for psychopathy (62,83) and ADHD (84). Finally, the findings from this Eastern European sample of drug users may not necessarily generalize to all cultures.

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of this paper.

Drug users continue to engage in sexual risk behaviors despite being in protracted abstinence, posing an ongoing public health risk, which appears to be driven in part by neurocognitive hot impulsivity and psychopathic traits. Risk behavior interventions within this population may benefit from cognitive remediation for hot impulsivity (e.g. 85,86) and interventions targeting psychopathy (e.g. 87–91). Future studies should systematically examine the relationships of neurocognitive impulsivity, externalizing psychopathology and sexual risk behaviors across different stages of the addiction cycle, including protracted abstinence. More precise neural correlates of hot impulsivity measures should be investigated via neuroimaging, as identifying substrates of hot impulsivity may provide incremental utility in predicting risk behavior and response to intervention.

Acknowledgements The authors thank all volunteers for their participation in this study and KB, RN, IR and GV for their assistance with data collection. Special thanks to RG for providing helpful feedback on the first draft of this manuscript.

Funding This study was completed with financial assistance from the Fogarty International Center (FIC) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) under award number R01DA021421 (J.V.). There was no involvement of a pharmaceutical or other private company in the funding of this research. Article sponsors (FIC/NIDA) played no role in the preparation of this manuscript. Study authors did not utilize any third party writing or editorial assistance in the preparation of this manuscript.

References 1. Aceijas C, Stimson GV, Hickman M, Rhodes T. Global overview of injecting drug use and HIV infection among injecting drug users. AIDS 2004;18:2295–2303. 2. Reekie J, Kowalska JD, Karpov I, Rockstroh J, Karlsson A, Rakhmanova A, Horban A, Kirk O, Lundgren JD, Mocroft A. Regional differences in AIDS and nonAIDS related mortality in HIV-positive individuals across Europe and Argentina: the EuroSIDA study. PloS One 2012;7:e41673.

THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF DRUG AND ALCOHOL ABUSE

3. Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS. Global report: UNAIDS report on the global AIDS epidemic; 2012. 4. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:1783–1793. 5. Masse LC, Tremblay RE. Behavior of boys in kindergarten and the onset of substance use during adolescence. Arch General Psychiatry 1997;54:62–68. 6. Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Newman DL, Silva PA. Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996;53:1033–1039. 7. Cloninger CR, Sigvardsson S, Bohman M. Childhood personality predicts alcohol abuse in young adults. Alcoholism: Clin Experimental Res 1988;12:494–505. 8. Biederman J, Wilens T, Mick E, Milberger S, Spencer TJ, Faraone SV. Psychoactive substance use disorders in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): effects of ADHD and psychiatric comorbidity. Am J Psychiatry 1995;152:1652–1658. 9. Krueger RF, Hicks BM, Patrick CJ, Carlson SR, Iacono WG, McGue M. Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior and personality: modeling the externalizing spectrum. J Abnormal Psychol 2002;111:411–424. 10. Chan YF, Dennis ML, Funk RR. Prevalence and comorbidity of major internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents and adults presenting to substance abuse treatment. J Substance Abuse Treatment 2008;34:14–24. 11. Kaufman JN, Ross TJ, Stein EA, Garavan H. Cingulate hypoactivity in cocaine users during a GO-NOGO task as revealed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 2003;23:7839–7843. 12. Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neurosci 2005;8:1458–1463. 13. Hester R, Garavan H. Executive dysfunction in cocaine addiction: evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate, and cerebellar activity. J Neurosci 2004;24:11017–11022. 14. Perry JL, Carroll ME. The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology 2008;200:1–26. 15. Fernandez-Serrano MJ, Perez-Garcia M, VerdejoGarcia A. What are the specific vs. generalized effects of drugs of abuse on neuropsychological performance? Neurosci Biobehavioral Rev 2011;35:377–406. 16. Badiani A, Belin D, Epstein D, Calu D, Shaham Y. Opiate versus psychostimulant addiction: the differences do matter. Nature Rev Neurosci 2011;12:685–700. 17. Clark L, Robbins TW, Ersche KD, Sahakian BJ. Reflection impulsivity in current and former substance users. Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:515–522. 18. Fleckenstein AE, Gibb JW, Hanson GR. Differential effects of stimulants on monoaminergic transporters: pharmacological consequences and implications for neurotoxicity. Eur J Pharmacol 2000;406:1–13. 19. Verdejo-Garcia A, Perez-Garcia M. Profile of executive deficits in cocaine and heroin polysubstance users: common and differential effects on separate executive components. Psychopharmacol 2007;190:517–530.

239

20. Kirby KN, Petry NM. Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non drug-using controls. Addiction 2004;99:461–471. 21. Bornovalova MA, Daughters SB, Hernandez GD, Richards JB, Lejuez CW. Differences in impulsivity and risk-taking propensity between primary users of crack cocaine and primary users of heroin in a residential substance-use program. Experim Clin Psychopharmacol 2005;13:311–318. 22. Bux DA, Lamb RJ, Iguchi MY. Cocaine use and HIV risk behavior in methadone maintenance patients. Drug Alcohol Dependence 1995;37:29–35. 23. Camacho LM, Bartholomew NG, Joe GW, Cloud MA, Simpson DD. Gender, cocaine and during-treatment HIV risk reduction among injection opioid users in methadone maintenance. Drug Alcohol Dependence 1996;41:1–7. 24. Lejuez CW, Bornovalova MA, Daughters SB, Curtin JJ. Differences in impulsivity and sexual risk behavior among inner-city crack/cocaine users and heroin users. Drug Alcohol Dependence 2005;77:169–175. 25. Ornstein TJ, Iddon JL, Baldacchino AM, Sahakian BJ, London M, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Profiles of cognitive dysfunction in chronic amphetamine and heroin abusers. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000;23:113–126. 26. Brand M, Roth-Bauer M, Driessen M, Markowitsch HJ. Executive functions and risky decision-making in patients with opiate dependence. Drug Alcohol Dependence 2008;97:64–72. 27. Fishbein DH, Krupitsky E, Flannery BA, Langevin DJ, Bobashev G, Verbitskaya E, Augustine CB, et al. Neurocognitive characterizations of Russian heroin addicts without a significant history of other drug use. Drug Alcohol Dependence 2007;90:25–38. 28. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain 2000;123:2189–2202. 29. Bickel WK, Marsch LA. Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. Addiction 2001;96:73–86. 30. Dougherty DM, Marsh DM, Mathias CW. Immediate and delayed memory tasks: a computerized behavioral measure of memory, attention, and impulsivity. Behavior Res Meth, Instruments Computers 2002;34:391–398. 31. Evenden JL. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology 1999;146:348–361. 32. Lane SD, Cherek DR, Rhoades HM, Pietras CJ, Tcheremissine OV. Relationships among laboratory and psychometric measures of impulsivity: implications in substance abuse and dependence. Addictive Disorders Their Treatment 2003;2:33–40. 33. Rogers RD, Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, Blackshaw AJ, Swainson R, Wynne K, Baker NB, et al. Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophandepleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999;20:322– 339.

240

M. J. WILSON & J. VASSILEVA

34. Sonuga-Barke EJ. Psychological heterogeneity in AD/ HD – a dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behav Brain Res 2002;130:29–36. 35. Bjork JM, Chen G, Hommer DW. Psychopathic tendencies and mesolimbic recruitment by cues for instrumental and passively obtained rewards. Biol Psychol 2012;89:408–415. 36. De Brito SA, Viding E, Kumari V, Blackwood N, Hodgins S. Cool and hot executive function impairments in violent offenders with antisocial personality disorder with and without psychopathy. PloS One 2013;8;e65566. 37. Rubia K. Cool inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: a review. Biol Psychiatry 2011;69: e69–87. 38. Goel V, Dolan RJ. Reciprocal neural response within lateral and ventral medial prefrontal cortex during hot and cold reasoning. Neuroimage 2003;20:2314–2321. 39. Kim S, Lee D. Prefrontal cortex and impulsive decision making. Biol Psychiatry 2011;69:1140–1146. 40. Mitchell MM, Severtson SG, Latimer WW. Interaction of cognitive performance and knowing someone who has died from AIDS on HIV sexual risk behaviors. AIDS Educat Prevent 2007;19:289–297. 41. Ames SL, Grenard JL, Stacy AW. Dual process interaction model of HIV-sexual risk behaviors among drug offenders. AIDS Behav 2013;17:914–925. 42. Gonzalez R, Vassileva J, Bechara A, Grbesic S, Sworowski L, Novak RM, Nunnally G, et al. The influence of executive functions, sensation seeking, and HIV serostatus on the risky sexual practices of substance-dependent individuals. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2005;11:121–131. 43. Vassileva J, Ahn WY, Weber KM, Busemeyer JR, Stout JC, Gonzalez R, Cohen MH. Computational modeling reveals distinct effects of HIV and history of drug use on decision-making processes in women. PloS One 2013;8:e68962. 44. Wardle MC, Gonzalez R, Bechara A, MartinThormeyer EM. Iowa Gambling Task performance and emotional distress interact to predict risky sexual behavior in individuals with dual substance and HIV diagnoses. J Clin Experim Neuropsychol 2010;32:1110–1121. 45. Compton WM, Cottler LB, Ben-Abdallah A, Cunningham-Williams R, Spitznagel EL. The effects of psychiatric comorbidity on response to an HIV prevention intervention. Drug Alcohol Dependence 2000;58:247–257. 46. Darke S, Kaye S, Finlay-Jones R. Antisocial personality disorder, psychopathy and injecting heroin use. Drug Alcohol Dependence 1998;52:63–69. 47. Flory K, Molina BS, Pelham J, Gnagy E, Smith B. Childhood ADHD predicts risky sexual behavior in young adulthood. J Clin Child Adolescent Psychol 2006;35:571–577. 48. Brooner RK, King VL, Kidorf M, Schmidt CW Jr, Bigelow GE. Psychiatric and substance use comorbidity among treatment-seeking opioid abusers. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54:71–80.

49. Slesnick N, Prestopnik J. Dual and multiple diagnosis among substance using runaway youth. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2005;31:179–201. 50. Vassileva J, Petkova P, Georgiev S, Martin EM, Tersiyski R, Raycheva M, Velinov V, et al. Impaired decision-making in psychopathic heroin addicts. Drug Alcohol Dependence 2007;86:287–289. 51. Vassileva J, Georgiev S, Martin E, Gonzalez R, Segala L. Psychopathic heroin addicts are not uniformly impaired across neurocognitive domains of impulsivity. Drug Alcohol Dependence 2011;114:194–200. 52. European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction (EMCDDA). European Drug Report; 2013. 53. Heilig M, Egli M, Crabbe JC, Becker HC. Review: acute withdrawal, protracted abstinence and negative affect in alcoholism: are they linked? Addiction Biol 2010;15:169–184. 54. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the ‘dark side’ of drug addiction. Nature Neurosci 2005;8:1442–1444. 55. Wang B, Shaham Y, Zitzman D, Azari S, Wise RA, You ZB. Cocaine experience establishes control of midbrain glutamate and dopamine by corticotropin-releasing factor: a role in stress-induced relapse to drug seeking. J Neurosci 2005;25:5389–5396. 56. Amirkhanian YA, Kelly JA, Kabakchieva E, McAuliffe TL, Vassileva S. Evaluation of a social network HIV prevention intervention program for young men who have sex with men in Russia and Bulgaria. AIDS Edu Prev 2003;15:205–220. 57. Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH. Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press; 2004. 58. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders Research Version (SCID-I). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute. Biometrics Research; 1996. 59. Petry NM. Reliability of drug users’ self-reported HIV sexual risk behaviors using a brief, 11-item scale. Subst Use Misuse 2001;36:1731–1747. 60. Navaline HA, Snider EC, Petro CJ, Tobin D, Metzger D, Alterman AI, Woody GE. Preparations for AIDS vaccine trials. An automated version of the Risk Assessment Battery (RAB): enhancing the assessment of sexual risk behaviors. AIDS Res Human Retroviruses 1993;10: S281–283. 61. Hart SD, Cox DN, Hare RD. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version; 1995. 62. Wilson MJ, Abramowitz C, Vasilev G, Bozgunov K, Vassileva J. Psychopathy in Bulgaria: the cross-cultural generalizability of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 2014;36:389–400. 63. Ward MF, Wender PH, Reimherr FW. The Wender Utah Rating Scale: an aid in the retrospective diagnosis of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1993;150:885–890. 64. Sahakian BJ, Morris RG, Evenden JL, Heald A, Levy R, Philpot M, RobbinsTW. A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson’s disease. Brain 1988;111:695– 718.

THE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF DRUG AND ALCOHOL ABUSE

65. Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exper Psychol: Gen 1999;128:78–87. 66. Ersche KD, Fletcher PC, Lewis SJG, Clark L, StocksGee G, London M, Deakin JB, et al. Abnormal frontal activations related to decision-making in current and former amphetamine and opiate dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology 2005;180;612–623. 67. Nees F, Tzschoppe J, Patrick CJ, Vollstädt-Klein S, Steiner S, Poustka L, Banaschewski T, et al. Determinants of early alcohol use in healthy adolescents: the differential contribution of neuroimaging and psychological factors. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011;37:986–995. 68. Lawrence AJ, Luty J, Bogdan NA, Sahakian BJ, Clark L. Problem gamblers share deficits in impulsive decisionmaking with alcohol-dependent individuals. Addiction 2009;104:1006–1015. 69. Passetti F, Clark L, Mehta MA, Joyce E, King M. Neuropsychological predictors of clinical outcome in opiate addiction. Drug Alcohol Dependence 2008;94:82–91. 70. Schreiber L, Grant JE, Odlaug BL. Emotion regulation and impulsivity in young adults. J Psychiatric Res 2012;46:651–658. 71. Lane SD, Moeller FG, Steinberg JL, Buzby M, Kosten TR. Performance of cocaine dependent individuals and controls on a response inhibition task with varying levels of difficulty. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2007;33:717–726. 72. Dougherty DM, Mathias CW, Marsh DM, Jagar AA. Laboratory behavioral measures of impulsivity. Behav Res Meth 2005;37:82–90. 73. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2013. 74. Bovasso GB, Alterman AI, Cacciola JS, Rutherford MJ. The prediction of violent and nonviolent criminal behavior in a methadone maintenance population. J Pers Dis 2002;16:360–373. 75. Alterman AI, Rutherford MJ, Cacciola JS, McKay JR, Boardman CR. Prediction of 7 months methadone maintenance treatment response by four measures of antisociality. Drug Alcohol Dependence 1998;49:217– 223. 76. Rutherford MJ, McKay JR, Alterman AI, Cacciola JS, Cook TG. The relationship of object relations and reality testing deficits to outcome status of methadone maintenance patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry 1996;37:347–354. 77. Gudonis LC, Derefinko K, Giancola PR. The treatment of substance misuse in psychopathic individuals: why heterogeneity matters. Subst Use Misuse 2009;44:1415–1433. 78. Simon SL, Dean AC, Cordova X, Monterosso JR, London ED. Methamphetamine dependence and neuropsychological functioning: evaluating change during

79.

80.

81.

82.

83.

84.

85.

86.

87.

88.

89.

90.

91.

241

early abstinence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2010;71:335– 344. Scott JC, Woods SP, Matt GE, Meyer RA, Heaton RK, Atkinson JH, Grant I. Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: a critical review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 2007;17:275–297. Rapeli P, Kivisaari R, Autti T, Kahkonen S, Puuskari V, Jokela O, Kalska H. Cognitive function during early abstinence from opioid dependence: a comparison to age, gender, and verbal intelligence matched controls. BMC Psychiatry 2006;6:9. Lee T, Zhou WH, Luo XJ, Yuen KS, Ruan XZ, Weng XC. Neural activity associated with cognitive regulation in heroin users: a fMRI study. Neurosci Lett 2005;382:211–216. Ahn WY, Vasilev G, Lee SH, Busemeyer JR, Kruschke JK, Bechara A, Vassileva J. Decision-making in stimulant and opiate addicts in protracted abstinence: evidence from computational modeling with pure users. Frontiers Psychol 2014;5:849. Zukauskiene R, Laurinavicius A, Cesniene I. Testing factorial structure and validity of the PCL:SV in Lithuanian prison population. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 2010;32:363–372. Goetz M, Yeh CB, Ondrejka I, Akay A, Herczeg I, Dobrescu I, Kim BN, et al. A 12-month prospective, observational study of treatment regimen and quality of life associated with ADHD in Central and Eastern Europe and Eastern Asia. J Attention Disorders 2012;16:44–59. Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, Hill PF, Baxter C. Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biol Psychiatry 2011;69:260–265. Baskin-Sommers AR, Curtin JJ, Newman JP. Altering the cognitive-affective dysfunctions of psychopathic and externalizing offender subtypes with cognitive remediation. Clin Psychol Sci 2015;3:45–57. Caldwell M, Skeem J, Salekin R, Van Rybroek G. Treatment response of adolescent offenders with psychopathy features a 2-year follow-up. Crim Justice Behav 2006;33:571–596. Caldwell MF, McCormick DJ, Umstead D, Van Rybroek GJ. Evidence of treatment progress and therapeutic outcomes among adolescents with psychopathic features. Crim Justice Behav 2007;34:573–587. Olver ME, Wong SC.Therapeutic responses of psychopathic sexual offenders: treatment attrition, therapeutic change, and long-term recidivism. J Consult Clin Psychol 2009;77:328–336. White SF, Frick PJ, Lawing K, Bauer D. Callousunemotional traits and response to functional family therapy in adolescent offenders. Behav Sci Law 2013;31:271–285. Skeem JL, Monahan J, Mulvey EP. Psychopathy, treatment involvement, and subsequent violence among civil psychiatric patients. Law Human Behav 2002;26;577–603.

Neurocognitive and psychiatric dimensions of hot, but not cool, impulsivity predict HIV sexual risk behaviors among drug users in protracted abstinence.

Impulsivity is an important risk factor for HIV risky drug and sexual behaviors. Research identifies hot (i.e. affectively-mediated, reward-based) and...
566B Sizes 1 Downloads 7 Views