Journal of Counseling Psychology 2014, Vol. 61, No. 4,513-520

© 2014 American Psychological Association 0022-0167/14/$ 12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cou0000035

Neurosciences of Infant Mental Health Development: Recent Findings and Implications for Counseling Psychology Adriana Sampaio

Karin Lifter

University of Minho, Braga

Northeastern University

This article is about the neural correlates of infant mental health and their correspondences to social emotional development. These correspondences are organized in terms of the definition of infant mental provided by Zero to Three (2001), centered on infants’ capacities regarding the experience and expression of emotions, interpersonal relationships, and learning. We conclude with implications of these correspondences for counseling psychology— namely, working with children’s caregivers to maximize children’s healthy social and emotional development. Keywords: infants, infant mental health, parent-child relationships, social development, emotional development Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cou0000035.supp

Brain development, which is a continuous process with increas­ ing specialization and differentiation, is correlated with what in­ fants and young children say and do, and what they and their caregivers bring to their reciprocal interactions. Recent research in cognitive neuroscience has been providing new insights about how the genetic, environmental, and experience factors shape brain development, in utero and throughout the life span. Early experi­ ences, especially caregiver-child interactions, matter (Zeanah & Zeanah, 2009). As described by the Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University (2012), “experiences ‘authorize’ ge­ netic instructions to be carried out and shape the formation of the (brain) circuits as they are being constructed” (p. 1). This article is about the neural correlates of infant mental health and their correspondences to social emotional development. We begin with a definition of infant mental health, taken from Zero to Three (Zero to Three, 2001), which centers on infants’ capacities regarding the experience and expression of emotions, interpersonal relationships, and learning. We then present several studies on the neural correlates of infant mental health.1 We conclude with im­ plications for practice and research in counseling psychology.

secure interpersonal relationships; and explore the environment and learn—all in the context of family, community, and cultural expectations for young children. Infant mental health is synony­ mous with healthy social and emotional development” (Zero to Three, 2001, p. 1). These three capacities provide a useful way to organize the emerging literature in cognitive neuroscience. Studies of infants have demonstrated that the neural substrates of these developing capacities can be uncovered by the use of noninvasive neuroimaging techniques as electroencephalogram (EEG)/eventrelated potentials (ERPs), psychophysiology, magnetic resonance imaging (structural and functional), positron emission tomography (PET), magnetoencephalography, and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS).

Neural Correlates of the Ability to Experience, Regulate, and Express Emotions Infants’ abilities to perceive, experience, and recognize emo­ tions, which allows them to engage with caregivers, have been studied mainly through behavioral and EEG/ERP studies.

Definition of Infant Mental Health

Perception, Experience, and Recognition of Emotions

Infant mental health, which is an interdisciplinary field, is centered on “the developing capacity of the child from birth to age three to experience, regulate, and express emotions; form close and

At birth, the neonate is rapidly able to perceive emotions by selecting significant and important information from human faces and voices. Infants are able to learn about the other’s emotion by integrating social-perceptual stimuli such as face and human voice in their internal states (e.g., imitation; O’Reilly & deHann, 2009) and by recognizing emotional states. Face specialization. The first signs of face specialization are present early after birth, including attention to eye gaze, face and face-like patterns. Attention to eye gaze emerges early after birth,

Adriana Sampaio, Neuropsychophysiology Lab, CIPsi, School of Psy­ chology, University of Minho, Braga; Karin Lifter, Department of Coun­ seling and Applied Educational Psychology, Northeastern University. We thank Sara Cruz for her help in the reviewing process. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Adri­ ana Sampaio, Neuropsychophysiology Lab, CIPsi, School of Psychol­ ogy, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal. E-mail: adriana.sampaio@ psi.uminho.pt

1 For a summary of the neural correlates of infant mental health, please see the supplemental materials. 513

514

SAMPAIO AND LIFTER

with infants preferring to look at faces with directed rather than averted eye gaze (Farroni, Csibra, Simion, & Johnson, 2002). Underlying neural correlates include enhanced amplitude of N170 component, which is evident in 4-month-old infants when attend­ ing to direct gaze. Consistent with this is a distributed brain cortical activation that overlaps the adult face-processing area (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Auditory specialization. The infant’s ability to perceive emo­ tions by selecting significant and important information from the human voice is also present at birth. Research has demonstrated that neonates prefer human speech to other auditory stimuli, spe­ cifically, the maternal voice (DeCasper & Fifer, 1980). ERP cor­ relates of the auditory processing of the infant’s mother’s voice are associated with a smaller P350 and increased N450 amplitudes, when compared to an unfamiliar voice (Purhonen, KilpelainenLees, Valkonen-Korhonen, Karhu, & Lehtonen, 2004). Recognition of different emotional states. There is evidence of an early ability of the neonate to recognize, discriminate, and imitate facial expressions (Field, Woodson, Greenberg, & Cohen, 1982). Infants are relatively consistent in discriminating happy expressions from other emotions (Kotsoni, de Haan, & Johnson, 2001). Studies have demonstrated that in 4-month-old infants, the Nc amplitude is increased to happy expressions (Farroni, Menon, Rigato, & Johnson, 2007). However, this pattern of preferential attention allocation to happy faces changes to fearful expressions at 7 months of age. Consistent behavioral and EEG/ERP evidence shows that infants around this age allocate more attention re­ sources to fearful emotional expressions, in addition to an increase in the Nc component to fearful faces (Kotsoni et al., 2001). Despite this converging behavioral and neural evidence showing an increasing maturation of auditory and facial emotional expres­ sion recognition, sensorial modalities are integrated in most social interactions. Integration of facial and vocal emotional information was assessed within 7-month-old infants while processing emo­ tionally congruent and incongruent face-voice pairs, using EEG. The authors showed emotionally incongruent pairs were associated with larger negative ERP component, and emotionally congruent pairs were associated with an increased positive ERP component, which suggested infants are able to recognize emotional content and affect across modalities (Grossmann, Striano, & Friederici, 2006).

Regulation of Emotions Early regulation of emotions is imposed by caregivers in a responsive caregiving environment. Studies have shown that in­ fants prefer an infant-directed (ID) speech (DeCasper & Fifer, 1980). High arousal positive vocal emotion contributes signifi­ cantly to the infant’s preference for ID speech (Corbeil, Trehub, & Peretz, 2013). Maternal speech is therefore central to infant atten­ tion, arousal, and regulation. Studies have shown differential psychophysiological data according to the affective emotional state of the ID speech, namely a deceleration of the infant heart rate to ID speech independent of affective content (Santesso, Schmidt, & Trainor, 2007). Studies have shown that emotional prosody is associated with different psychophysiological and EEG signatures. More intense startle responses were documented in 5-month-old infants when exposed to angry facial emotional expressions (Balaban, 1995).

EEG/ERP studies have revealed that angry prosody elicits a more negative ERP component than does happy or neutral prosody in 7-month-old infants (Grossmann, Striano, & Friederici, 2005). Increased EEG power in the frontal regions of 9-month-old infants was documented in response to fear, as opposed to surprise and love/comfort (Santesso et al., 2007). Infants’ neural processing to emotional tone of voice, portrayed by non-physical interparental conflict, has been assessed with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in 6- to 12-monthold infants (Graham, Fisher, & Pfeifer, 2013). Results have shown that higher inter-parental conflict (very angry tone of voice) was associated with increased activation of the rostral anterior cingu­ late cortex, as well as subcortical regions— brain regions associ­ ated with emotional regulation and stress. These data suggest a greater allocation of brain and attention resources to negative emotions such as anger and fear.

Expression of Emotions Electrophysiological studies have suggested a lateralized brain response underlying the expression of positive and negative emo­ tional facial expressions. This cerebral asymmetry was demon­ strated in newborns when exposed to stimulus-eliciting affective responses. Fox and Davidson (1988) found that EEG frontal acti­ vation asymmetry could discriminate a range of positive and negative facial expressions in 10-month-old infants: specifically, greater left frontal relative activation in response to mother and happy stimuli, when compared with strangers and sad stimuli, respectively (Davidson & Fox, 1982; Fox & Davidson, 1988). By 18-21 months, toddlers begin to demonstrate awareness of themselves, which includes the expression of the social emotions (e.g., shame, guilt, pride). They also display increases in symbolic representation, as a function of developments in cognition (Rosenblum, Dayton, & Muzik, 2009). However, more studies on the perception and expression of these discrete expressions in infancy are clearly warranted in order to understand early emotional de­ velopment. In sum, these experience-dependent capacities that infants dem­ onstrate allow them to engage with the caregivers in their envi­ ronments, setting the stage for reciprocal interactions.

Neural Correlates of the Ability to Form Close and Secure Interpersonal Relationships Close and secure positive interpersonal relationships are central to infant mental health, forming the basis for attachment. They contribute to the development of prosocial behaviors.

Neural Correlates in the Development of Attachment As we have discussed, the infant is bom with the capacity to engage the environment, and in particular, to engage caregivers. The infant is able to focus and follow stimuli, being preferentially oriented toward human forms and sounds, when compared with non-social-oriented stimuli (Legerstee, Anderson, & Schaffer, 1998). These capacities are nurtured and encouraged by a respon­ sive caregiving environment; it is the moment that both parent and infant are attracted to each other, seeking contact and proximity that lead the dyad to establish affect-regulative interactions, where the mother-infant interaction is privileged.

NEUROSCIENCES OF INFANT MENTAL HEALTH DEVELOPMENT

Social and emotional developments contribute to the transac­ tions between infants and caregivers, setting the stage for caregiver responsiveness to the infant’s developing capacities. A secure attachment style provides a platform for exploring the environ­ ment. Around 9 months of age, the foundations for higher socioemotional and cognitive functions are being built as the infant develops the sense of understanding subjective states of others as well as acting jointly. These processes are threatened when the caregiver is depressed or unable to respond sensitively (Murray, Fiori-Crowley, Hooper, & Cooper, 1996). A synchronous caregiver-infant relationship is associated with optimal parenting, and it is predicted by cardiac vagal tone (Feld­ man & Eidelman, 2007). Higher affect synchrony and infant social engagement are also associated with increased peripheral oxytocin measures (Feldman, Gordon, & Zagoory-Sharon, 2010). Studies have supported a dynamic interplay between the maturation of the “social brain,” involving the orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala, and temporal regions and the establishment of an attachment relation­ ship (Moriceau & Sullivan, 2005). However, the role of these brain regions in mother-infant attachment has been studied mainly from the mother’s perspective; limited information is available on the infant’s brain areas underlying the development of attachment relationships. In fact, most studies in the neurobiology of infant attachment have been conducted in animal models (see Moriceau & Sullivan, 2005). Indirect evidence of infant brain development documents sub­ sequent increases in glucose uptake in these attachment-related brain regions, including frontal and various association regions, around 8 months (Chugani, 1998). A cortical surface expansion of the superior and medial temporal, superior parietal, medial orbito­ frontal, lateral anterior prefrontal, occipital cortices, and postcen­ tral gyrus areas has been observed (Li et al., 2013). These func­ tional and structural changes correspond to the precursors of theory of mind, thinking, self-referential activity, and internal regulation. They correspond to the emerging understanding of persons and objects as permanent entities and to the ability to use symbols in the representation of experience.

Neural Correlates in the Disruption of Responsive Caregiving Studies have demonstrated neurological impacts as a result of less than optimal caregiving (e.g., Hanson et al., 2013; Stratheam, 2011). Neglect has been identified as “the most prevalent form of child maltreatment” (National Scientific Council on the Develop­ ing Child, 2012, p. 6). Events such as “abandonment . . . by the parent; early separation or loss of the primary care-giving figure; early exposure to trauma; abuse (and neglect); maternal depression and parental psychopathology; conflicted family relationships; se­ vere lack of resources; premature birth; difficult infant tempera­ ment; and poor supplemental care settings” (Fitzgerald, Weatherston, & Mann, 2011, p. 181) are all threats. Studies have demonstrated that children who are neglected show atypical EEG power distribution, including lower absolute alpha power (Tarullo, Garvin, & Gunnar, 2011), as well as abnormal activity in the amygdala and hippocampus as a consequence of neglect (Totten­ ham et al., 2011). Infants of depressed mothers experience more negative expressions than do infants of non-depressed mothers, showing altered EEG signature, namely greater relative right fron­

515

tal activation (Diego, Field, Jones, & Hernandez-Reif, 2006). The emotional environment provided by the mother’s affective dispo­ sition is associated with the infant’s processing of facial expres­ sions of emotion (de Haan, Belsky, Reid, Volein, & Johnson, 2004). Studies conducted in infants who were maltreated, neglected, and abused show a deleterious impact in both mental and physical health development. Indeed, atypical rearing condi­ tions are believed to hinder the development of stable relation­ ships (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., 2011) and to inhibit the normative shift in socio-emotional development during the first years of life. Moreover, difficulties with responsive caregiving often co-occur with other threats to family and community life (Murray et al., 1999).

Neural Correlates of the Ability to Explore the Environment and Learn In order to explore the environment and learn, the infant needs to develop the social and emotional skills (i.e., attachment) to create a secure base for exploration. The infant also needs to integrate multisensory stimuli into internal and self-regulatory states and to develop motor skills in order to move about— both moving in relation to the environment and moving objects in relation to the stationery self and others. Finally, the infant needs to develop cognitive and social communicative skills—to acquire knowledge about objects, people, and events and the relationships among them and to express that knowledge in his or her language and play.

Neural Correlates of Multisensorial Integration The neonate is already able to apprehend and explore the envi­ ronment, through multiple sensory cues (e.g., olfactory, somato­ sensory, visual, and auditory). Salient cues include the perception of the mother’s odor, as olfactory learning occurs during the first hours of postnatal life. Functional data (NIRS) have shown that in the 6 hr to 192 hr after birth, the neonate demonstrates a differ­ ential pattern of changes in blood flow over the left orbitofrontal region when exposed to vanilla and colostrum smell (Bartocci et al., 2000). Greater activation of the orbitofrontal cortex to the odor of maternal breast milk compared to formula milk was demon­ strated (Aoyama et al., 2010). Human tactile stimulation was reported to be associated with improvement in certain biological and behavioral conditions, by promoting body and mental development. Several studies exam­ ining electric potentials and magnetic fields (supplementary eye fields) showed consistent neural responses (N1-P1-P2 and M60) distributed from posterior to anterior regions, generated in the primary cortex (e.g., Nevalainen et al., 2008). fMRI evidence has revealed an activation of the contralateral primary somatosensory cortex to tactile stimulation (Arichi et al., 2010). These markers, found in healthy, full-term newborns, corresponded to changes in developmental processes (Gois-Eanes, Gonsalves, Caldeira-daSilva, & Sampaio, 2012). Although the neonate's ability to differentiate visual details is limited, the visual system is characterized by substantial changes in the first year: a fast increase in synaptic density in the visual cortex in parallel with intense myelination of the visual tracts in

516

SAMPAIO AND LIFTER

the first four postnatal months (Dubois et al., 2008). At 1 month of age, early visual evoked potential components, such as N l, PI and N2, play an important role in healthy brain development, as the presence of these components is associated with healthy visual stimuli processing (Benavente, Tamargo, Tajada, Yuste, & Olivan, 2005). By 6 months of age, healthy full-term born infants show a positive P2 component associated with good neural maturation of the neonatal visual cortex (Benavente et al., 2005). One of the earliest functions to emerge is the auditory function. In fact, the P I50, N250, P350, and N450 peak components were identifiable both at birth and at 12 months (Kushnerenko et al., 2002). However, the maturation of these components is considered to be a continual process throughout infancy, as the amplitude of the peaks increase and the latency of the peaks decrease through­ out time (Wunderlich & Cone-Wesson, 2006). All these brain maturational processes are corroborated by an increase in the glucose uptake in the parietal, temporal, and primary visual cortex, among other brain regions, at 2-3 months (Chugani, 1998). Multisensorial integration allows the infant to pick up and explore toys (around 6 months), to create relationships between and among toys (around 12 months), and to represent his or her experiences in play activities (around 18 months; Lifter & Bloom, 1989). These developments depend upon the maturation of specific neuromotor networks in parallel with specialized brain circuits involved in motor planning, anticipation, motor imagery, and spatial cognition. They are strongly associated with cognitive, emotional, and language skills (Koziol & Lutz, 2013).

Neural Correlates of Motor Development At birth, the motor behavior repertoire consists of an array of neurological reflexes (e.g., grasping, sucking) and specific combi­ nations of limb and muscle movements (flexors activity, extensor activity). From birth to about 3 -4 months of age, in parallel with advancements of the visual system, postural trunk control, eyehand coordination, and arm velocity and muscle forces regulation, infants develop a phase of pre-reaching movements (AnguloBarroso & Tiernan, 2008). Increased glucose metabolism in the primary sensorimotor cortex, cingulate cortex, thalamus, brain stem, cerebellum, and hippocampal region has been described in the newborn (Chugani, 1998). These results are consistent with activity of the resting state networks (e.g., sensorimotor) detected in the infant brain (Fransson et al., 2007). Maturation of these brain networks is associated with adjustments in motor behavior but also in motivation, attention, anticipation, learning about current and prospective control of movement, motor planning, motor memory, and recognition of motor actions. This maturation is modulated by the experience; the infant’s experiences will have a structural and functional impact in the development of these neuromotor systems (Angulo-Barroso & Tiernan, 2008).

Neural Correlates of Language and Cognitive Development Joint attention emerges at around 9-10 months of age and is considered an important milestone for language and socialemotional development (Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998). Neural signatures underlying infant attention allocation (increased Nc amplitude) were evident when 9-month-old infants viewed

objects after an interactive joint attention task compared to a non-joint attention interaction (Striano, Reid, & Hoehl, 2006). The neural precursors (e.g., left dorsal prefrontal cortex) are evident at the age of 5 months (Grossmann & Johnson, 2010). Consistent with this neurobiological readiness for sharing attention, it was observed that the white matter integrity of the right fronto-limbic connection (uncinate fasciculus) at 6 months predicted infants’ responding to joint attention at 9 months (Elison et al., 2013). Joint attention is an important predictor of socio-communicative skills, with these fronto-temporal networks involved in the later development of language skills. Maturation of the auditory cortex is associated with early language learning skills (e.g., speech sound discrimination). Infants at 3 months are able to discriminate speech presented in normal and reversed order, exhibiting greater func­ tional activation of left temporal areas (similar to adults) to speech stimuli and a right frontal activation when processing normal speech (Dehaene-Lambertz, Dehaene, & Hertz-Pannier, 2002). These results are consistent with other fMRI and magneto­ encephalography studies showing an activation of speech produc­ tion brain areas when infants are processing speech-related stimuli (Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2006; Imada et al., 2006). There is evidence of an increasing synchronized perceptualmotor brain activity when listening to speech, as activation of both auditory and motor brain regions is only detected in 6- and 12-month-old infants and not in newborns (Imada et al., 2006). Infants at 12-18 months recruited adult-like left fronto­ temporal areas necessary for processing lexico-semantic infor­ mation (understanding new and old words; Travis et al., 2011). This knowledge about word meanings and underlying ERP signature (N400m) is possibly related to an infant’s ability to produce words, with a marked spurt in vocabulary evident around 18 months (Lifter & Bloom, 1989). Moreover, increased neural efficiency is associated with language proficiency, with ERP components to comprehended and unknown words being more widely distributed over bilateral anterior and posterior brain regions in 13- to 17-month-old infants compared with more focal and limited activation of left temporal and parietal regions at 20 months (Mills, Coffey-Corina, & Neville, 1997). Toddlers are also likely to be sensitive to syntactic violations in spoken sentences, showing an early left-lateralized brain re­ sponse when an expected verb is wrongly replaced by a noun (Bernal, Dehaene-Lambertz, Millotte, & Christophe, 2010).

Implications for Practice and Research in Counseling Psychology Implications for practice and research include working with families to support responsive care, and examining the impact of interventions on behavior and underlying neural correlates.

Implications for Practice: Working With Families to Support Responsive Care This work would begin with practitioners being alert to threats to responsive care, which would include neglect and abuse by caregivers, depression and psychopathology of care­ givers, a severe lack of resources for the family, and vulnera­ bilities imposed by the infants (e.g., prematurity, difficult tem­ peraments; Fitzgerald et al., 2011).

NEUROSCIENCES OF INFANT MENTAL HEALTH DEVELOPMENT

Once families who are risk are identified, interventions can be put into place. In their meta-analysis of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoom, and Juffer (2003) concluded enhancing parental sen­ sitivity plays a causal role in enhancing attachment security. They reviewed various interventions that increase maternal sensitivity, concluding that effective interventions require a moderate number of sessions with clear behavioral focus (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al„ 2003). Fortunately, developmental outcomes for children who have been neglected may be changed through counseling interventions. Improvements in quality of care have been found to promote security (van den Boom, 1994; van den Dries, Juffer, Van IJzen­ doom, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2009). Strategies exist for working with caregivers to improve their relationships with their children, and as noted in the Bakermans-Kranenburg et al. (2003) review, relatively short behavioral interventions focused on ma­ ternal sensitivity were highly effective. Strategies include working with caregivers to build skills, provide support, and provide pos­ itive interactions (Tomlin & Viehweg, 2003) and focusing on working with the family as opposed to using interventions that focus on the child alone. Heffron (2000) recommended services along a continuum of support. Included were more globally based services that promote community health, which could involve working with pediatricians to identify families in which the infant-caregiver relationship is compromised (Heffron, 2000). More individual levels could in­ clude relationship-based support strategies that include the provi­ sion of general support and education to new parents. Still more intensive support would involve psychotherapeutic interventions for caregivers who suffer from depression, for example, and are unable to provide responsive caregiving. Interventions have been identified for children who have suffered from neglect, and a number have focused on the resilience of infants. Social, emotional, and environmental de­ privation has been associated with atypical brain development, emotional problems, and increased risk for psychopathology (McLaughlin, Fox, Zeanah, & Nelson, 2011). These deleterious effects exert their maximal influence during sensitive periods of development where there is maximal effect on the developing brain. Windows of vulnerability to experience-dependent changes may be mitigated by specific preventive interventions in order to potentiate flexible human adaptational systems that allow infants to be resilient (Masten, 2001). There is also evidence that some children are more affected by their envi­ ronmental experiences than others (Belsky, BakermansKranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2007).

Implications for Research The need for developing, implementing, and testing effective interventions continues to be important. A major line of re­ search is in child-parent psychotherapy (CCP; Lieberman & Van Horn, 2009). CPP centers on the premise that “nurturance, protection, and culturally and age appropriate socialization from the attachment figure(s) comprise the cornerstone of men­ tal health in infancy and early childhood and create interactive patterns that are internalized by the child in the forms of stable

517

and lifelong psychological “structures” (Lieberman & Van Horn, 2009; p. 439). Another empirically validated clinical model is the Circle of Security (Powell, Cooper, Hoffman, & Marvin, 2009), which is an attachment-based method to help caregivers provide a secure base for their infants. Still other interventions focus on the education of the caregivers, especially if they are compromised in some way (Suchman, DeCoste, & Mayes, 2009). Given the use and validation of such interventions, it will be important to see change in the neural correlates—for both care­ givers and children—that result from them, in addition to behav­ ioral outcomes. Currently, outcomes are measured in behavioral terms (Lieberman & Van Horn. 2009) and in improvements in attachment scores (Powell et al., 2009). Although there are substantial data on the infant’s ability to experience and express emotions and explore the environment and learn, research on the neurobiology of infant attachment consists mainly of studies from the mother’s perspective or based on the use of animal models. Therefore, an important line of research would be to investigate in infants the neural corre­ lates of forming close and secure attachment relationships. More studies using neuroimaging methods for unraveling the neural correlates of infant mental health should be encouraged. In fact, few studies are available partially due to the fact that neuroimaging methods (e.g., positron emission tomography, fMRI) are challenging to use in healthy infants. New fMRI protocols as well as the advent of multichannel NIRS, however, may overcome these limitations. Another line of research is to include other neurobiological markers (e.g., oxytocin) and per­ form genetic and epigenetic studies in order to assess how genetic expression can be regulated by the environment and “translated” into different neural and behavioral phenotypes through a Gene X Environment (GXE) perspective. Finally, several studies of infants show the brain’s ability for experience-dependent plasticity (Cramer et al., 2011), with specific associative brain areas (e.g., the prefrontal cortex) being highly modifiable by cognitive and affective experiences. Interventions have demonstrated increased nurturing in the caregivers as well as improved self-regulation and more orga­ nized attachment in the children (Dozier, Dozier, & Manni, 2002). The National Scientific Council on the Developing Child (2012) provides an overview of promising intervention models that have demonstrated psychosocial (e.g., more secure attach­ ments) and neurological (e.g., more typical patterns of cortisol production) impacts. Obviously, more studies are needed.

Conclusions Although this article is not an exhaustive review of the literature on neural correlates, the correspondences we pre­ sented suggest important implications for practice and research. It is critical for counseling psychologists to recognize the nature of these neural correlates and their impact on development when caregiving is less than optimal. Counseling psychologists are in a pivotal role in their work with individuals, families, and the community to identify caregivers who are unable to deliver responsive care. They also play central roles in working with personnel in community agencies (e.g., Early Intervention Cen­ ters, Head Start) to develop and sustain policy and system

518

SAMPAIO AND LIFTER

efforts to aid families in the care of their children. Future research should continue to investigate the neural correlates of what children say and do, and what their caregivers say and do, in addition to the impact of these interventions on the devel­ oping brain.

References Angulo-Barroso, R. M., & Tieman, C. H. (2008). Motor systems develop­ ment. In C. H. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook o f developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 147-160). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Aoyama, S., Toshima, T., Saito, Y., Konishi, N., Motoshige, K., Ishikawa, N .......... Kobayashi, M. (2010). Maternal breast milk odour induces frontal lobe activation in neonates: A NIRS study. Early Human Devel­ opment, 86, 541-545. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.07.003 Arichi, T., Moraux, A., Melendez, A., Doria, V., Groppo, M., Merchant, N., . . . Edwards, A. D. (2010). Somatosensory cortical activation identified by functional MRI in preterm and term infants. Neuroimage, 49, 2063-2071. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.038 Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Steele, H., Zeanah, C. H., Muhamedrahimov, R. J., Vorria, P., Dobrova-Krol, N. A .,. . . Gunnar, M. R. (2011). HI. Attachment and emotional development in institutional care: Char­ acteristics and catch up. Monographs o f the Society fo r Research in Child Development, 76, 62-91. doi:10.1111/j. 1540-5834.2011.00628.x Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoom, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 195-215. doi: 10.1037/ 0033-2909.129.2.195 Balaban, M. T. (1995). Affective influences on startle in five-month-old infants: Reactions to facial expressions of emotions. Child Development, 66, 28-36. doi: 10.2307/1131188 Bartocci, M., Winberg, J., Ruggiero, C., Bergqvist, L. L., Serra, G., & Lagercrantz, H. (2000). Activation of olfactory cortex in newborn in­ fants after odor stimulation: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. P ediatric Research, 48, 18-23. doi: 10.1203/00006450200007000-00006 Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoom, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300-304. doi: 10.1111/j. 1467-8721,2007.00525.x Benavente, I., Tamargo, P., Tajada, N., Yuste, V., & Olivan, M. J. (2005). Flash visually evoked potentials in the newborn and their maturation during the first six months of life. Documenta Ophthalmologica, 110, 255-263. doi: 10.1007/s 10633-005-0818-0 Bernal, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Millotte, S., & Christophe, A. (2010). Two-year-olds compute syntactic structure on-line. Developmental Sci­ ence, 13, 69-76. doi: 10.1111/j. 1467-7687.2009.00865.x Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs o f the Society fo r Research in Child Development, 63, i—174. doi: 10.2307/1166214 Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University. (2012). The science o f neglect: The persistent absence o f responsive care disrupts the de­ veloping brain (Working Paper 12). Retrieved from www.developingchild.harvard.edu Chugani, H. T. (1998). A critical period of brain development: Studies of cerebral glucose utilization with PET. Preventive Medicine, 27, 184— 188. doi: 10.1006/pmed. 1998.0274 Corbeil, M., Trehub, S. E., & Peretz, I. (2013). Speech vs. singing: Infants choose happier sounds. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 372. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00372 Cramer, S. C., Sur, M., Dobkin, B. H., O’Brien, C., Sanger, T. D., Trojanowski, J. Q., . . . Vinogradov, S. (2011). Harnessing neuroplas­

ticity for clinical applications. Brain, 134, 1591-1609. doi: 10.1093/ brain/awr039 Davidson, R. J., & Fox, N. A. (1982, December 17). Asymmetrical brain activity discriminates between positive and negative affective stimuli in human infants. Science, 218, 1235-1237. doi: 10.1126/science.7146906 DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980, June 6). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208, 1174-1176. doi: 10.1126/science.7375928 de Haan, M„ Belsky, J., Reid, V., Volein, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2004). Maternal personality and infants’ neural and visual responsivity to facial expressions of emotion. Journal o f Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1209-1218. doi: 10.111 l/j.!469-7610.2004.00320.x Dehaene-Lambertz, G.. Dehaene, S., & Hertz-Pannier, L. (2002, December 6). Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science, 298, 2013-2015. doi: 10.1126/science. 1077066 Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Hertz-Pannier, L„ Dubois, J., Meriaux, S., Roche, A., Sigman, M., & Dehaene, S. (2006). Functional organization of perisylvian activation during presentation of sentences in preverbal infants. Proceedings o f the National Academy o f Sciences USA, 103, 14240-14245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0606302103 Diego, M. A., Field, T., Jones, N. A., & Hernandez-Reif, M. (2006). Withdrawn and intrusive maternal interaction style and infant frontal EEG asymmetry shifts in infants of depressed and non-depressed moth­ ers. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 220-229. doi: 10.1016/j .infbeh.2005.12.002 Dozier, M., Dozier, D., & Manni, M. (2002). Attachment and biobehavioral catch-up: The ABCs of helping infants in foster care cope with early adversity. Zero to Three, 22, 7-13. Dubois, J., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Soares, C., Cointepas, Y., Le Bihan, D.. & Hertz-Pannier, L. (2008). Microstructural correlates of infant func­ tional development: Example of the visual pathways. Journal o f Neu­ roscience, 28, 1943-1948. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5145-07.2008 Elison, J. T., Wolff, J. J.. Heimer, D. C., Paterson, S. J., Gu, H., Hazlett, H. C .,. . . The IBIS Network. (2013). Frontolimbic neural circuitry at 6 months predicts individual differences in joint attention at 9 months. Developmental Science, 16, 186-197. doi: 10.1111/desc. 12015 Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings o f the National Academy o f Sciences USA, 99, 9602-9605. doi: 10.1073/pnas. 152159999 Farroni, T., Menon, E., Rigato, S., & Johnson, M. H. (2007). The percep­ tion of facial expressions in newborns. European Journal o f Develop­ mental Psychology, 4, 2-13. doi: 10.1080/17405620601046832 Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2007). Maternal postpartum behavior and the emergence of infant-mother and infant-father synchrony in preterm and full-term infants: The role of neonatal vagal tone. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 290-302. doi:10.1002/dev.20220 Feldman, R., Gordon, I., & Zagoory-Sharon, O. (2010). The cross­ generation transmission of oxytocin in humans. Hormones and Behav­ ior, 58, 669-676. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.06.005 Field, T. M., Woodson, R„ Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982, October 8). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218, 179-181. doi: 10.1126/science.7123230 Fitzgerald, H. E., Weatherston, D., & Mann, T. L. (2011). Infant mental health: An interdisciplinary framework for early social and emotional development. Current Problems in Pediatric Adolescent Health Care, 41, 178-182. doi:10.1016/j.cppeds.2011.02.001 Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1988). Patterns of brain electrical activity during facial signs of emotion in 10-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 24, 230-236. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.2.230 Fransson, P., Skiold, B., Horsch, S., Nordell, A., Blennow, M., Lager­ crantz, H., & Aden, U. (2007). Resting-state networks in the infant brain. Proceedings o f the National Academy o f Sciences USA, 104, 1553115536. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704380104

NEUROSCIENCES OF INFANT MENTAL HEALTH DEVELOPMENT Gois-Eanes, M.. Gon9alves, 6 . F., Caldeira-da-Silva, P., & Sampaio, A. (2012) . Biological and physiological markers of tactile sensorial pro­ cessing in healthy newborns. Infant Mental Health Journal, 33, 535542. doi:10.1002/imhj.21328 Graham, A. M., Fisher, P. A., & Pfeifer, J. H. (2013). What sleeping babies hear: An fMRI study of interparental conflict and infants’ emotion processing. Psychological Science, 24, 782-789. doi: 10.1177/ 0956797612458803 Grossmann, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2010). Selective prefrontal cortex responses to joint attention in early infancy. Biology Letters, 6, 540-543. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.1069 Grossmann, T., Striano, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Infants’ electric brain responses to emotional prosody. NeuroReport, 16, 1825-1828. doi: 10.1097/01. wnr.0000185964.34336.bl Grossmann, T., Striano, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Crossmodal inte­ gration of emotional information from face and voice in the infant brain. Developmental Science, 9, 309-315. doi: 10.I l l 1/j. 1467-7687.2006 ,00494.x Hanson, J. L., Adluru, N„ Chung, M. K., Alexander, A. L., Davidson, R. J., 6 Poliak, S. D. (2013). Early neglect is associated with alterations in white matter integrity and cognitive functioning. Child Development, 84, 1566-1578. Heffron, M. C. (2000). Clarifying concepts on infant mental health: Pro­ motion, relationship-based preventive intervention, and treatment. In­ fants & Young Children, 12, 14-21. doi: 10.1097/00001163-20001204000007 Imada, T., Zhang, Y., Cheour, M., Taulu, S., Ahonen, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2006). Infant speech perception activates Broca’s area: A developmen­ tal magnetoencephalography study. NeuroReport, 17, 957-962. doi: 10.1097/01 .wnr.0000223387.51704.89 Kotsoni, E., de Haan, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2001). Categorical perception of facial expressions by 7-month-old infants. Perception, 30, 1115— 1125. doi:10.1068/p3155 Koziol, L. F., & Lutz, J. T. (2013). From movement to thought: The development of executive function. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 2, 104-115. doi:10.1080/21622965.2013.748386 Kushnerenko, E., Ceponiene, R„ Balan, P., Fellman, V., Huotilaine, M., & Naatane, R. (2002). Maturation of the auditory event-related potentials during the first year of life. NeuroReport, 13, 47-51. doi: 10.1097/ 00001756-200201210-00014 Legerstee, M., Anderson, D., & Schaffer, A. (1998). Five- and eightmonth-old infants recognize their faces and voices as familiar and social stimuli. Child Development, 69, 37-50. doi: 10.1111/j. 1467-8624.1998 .tb06131.x Li, G., Nie, J., Wang, L., Shi, F., Lin, W., Gilmore, J. H., & Shen, D. (2013) . Mapping region-specific longitudinal cortical surface expansion from birth to 2 years of age. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 2724-2733. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs265 Lieberman, A. F., & Van Horn, P. (2009). Child-parent psychotherapy: A developmental approach to mental health treatment in infancy and early childhood. In C. J. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook o f infant mental health (3rd ed., pp. 439-449). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Lifter, K., & Bloom, L. (1989). Object play and the emergence of language. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 395-423. doi: 10.1016/01636383(89)90023-4 Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in develop­ ment. American Psychologist, 56, 227-238. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56 .3.227 McLaughlin, K. A., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Nelson, C. A. (2011). Adverse rearing environments and neural development in children: The development of frontal electroencephalogram asymmetry. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 1008-1015. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.08.006 Mills, D. M., Coffey-Corina, S., & Neville, H. J. (1997). Language com­ prehension and cerebral specialization from 13 to 20 months. Develop­

519

mental Neuropsychology, 13, 397-445. doi: 10.1080/8756564970 9540685 Moriceau, P., & Sullivan, R. M. (2005). Neurobiology of infant attach­ ment. Developmental Psychobiology, 47, 230-242. Murray, L., Fiori-Crowley, A., Hooper, R., & Cooper, P. (1996). The impact of postnatal depression and associated anxiety on early motherinfant interactions and later infant outcome. Child Development, 67, 2512-2526. doi: 10.2307/1131637 Murray, L., Sinclair, D., Cooper, P., Ducournau, P., Turner, P., & Stein, A. (1999). The socioemotional development of 5-year-old children of postnatally depressed mothers. Journal o f Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 1259-1271. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00542 Nevalainen, P., Lauronen, L., Sambeth, A., Wikstrom, H., Okada, Y., & Pihko, E. (2008). Somatosensory evoked magnetic fields from the pri­ mary and secondary somatosensory cortices in healthy newborns. Neurolmage, 40, 738-745. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.075 O’Reilly, H., & deHann, M. (2009). The neural basis of face processing in infancy and its relationship to the development of empathy. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 4, 429-448. Powell, B., Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., & Marvin, R. S. (2009). The circle of security. In C. H. J. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook o f infant mental health (3rd ed., pp. 450-467). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Purhonen, M., Kilpelainen-Lees, R„ Valkonen-Korhonen, M.. Karhu, J., & Lehtonen, J. (2004). Cerebral processing of mother’s voice compared to unfamiliar voice in 4-month-old infants. International Journal o f Psy­ chophysiology, 52, 257-266. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.11.003 Rosenblum, K. L., Dayton, C. J., & Muzik, M. (2009). Infant social and emotional development: Emerging competence in a relational context. In C. H. J. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook o f infant mental health (3rd ed., pp. 80-103). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Santesso, D. L., Schmidt, L. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infantdirected (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants. Brain and Cognition, 65, 14-21. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.008 Stratheam, L. (2011). Maternal neglect: Oxytocin, dopamine and the neurobiology of attachment. Journal o f Neuroendocrinology, 23, 10541065. Striano, T., Reid, V. M., & Hoehl, S. (2006). Neural mechanisms of joint attention in infancy. European Journal o f Neuroscience, 23, 2819-2823. doi: 10.111 l/j,1460-9568.2006.04822.x Suchman, N., DeCoste, C., & Mayes, L. (2009). The Mothers and Toddlers Program: An attachment-based intervention for mothers in substance abuse treatment. In C. H. J. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook o f infant mental health (pp. 485-499). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Tarullo, A. R., Garvin, M. C., & Gunnar, M. R. (2011). Atypical EEG power correlates with indiscriminately friendly behavior in internation­ ally adopted children. Developmental Psychology, 47, 417-431. doi: 10.1037/a0021363 Tomlin, A. M., & Viehweg, S. A. (2003). Infant mental health: Making a difference. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34, 617— 625. doi: 10.1037/0735-7028.34.6.617 Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Millner, A., Gilhooly, T., Zevin, J. D., & Casey, B. J. (2011). Elevated amygdala response to faces following early deprivation. Developmental Science, 14, 190-204. doi: 10.1111/j. 14677687.2010.00971.x Travis, K. E., Leonard, M. K., Brown, T. T., Hagler, D. J., Jr., Curran, M., Dale, A. M ........Halgren, E. (2011). Spatiotemporal neural dynamics of word understanding in 12- to 18-month-old infants. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 1832-1839. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq259 Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., De Schonen, S., Crivello, F., Reutter, B., Aujard, Y., & Mazoyer, B. (2002). Neural correlates of woman face processing by 2-month-old infants. Neuroimage, 15, 454-461. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001 .0979

520

SAMPAIO AND LIFTER

van den Boom, D. C. (1994). The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensi­ tive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants. Child Development, 65, 1457-1477. doi: 10.2307/1131511 van den Dries, L., Juffer, F., Van IJzendoom, M. H„ & BakermansKranenburg, M. J. (2009). Fostering security? A meta-analysis of at­ tachment in adopted children. Children and Youth Services Review, 31, 410-421. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2008.09.008 Wunderlich, J. L., & Cone-Wesson, B. K. (2006). Maturation of CAEP in infants and children: A review. Hearing Research, 212, 212-223. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2005.11.008

Zeanah, C. H. J., & Zeanah, P. D. (2009). The scope of infant mental health. In C. H. J. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook o f infant mental health (3rd ed„ pp. 5-21). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Zero to Three. (2001). Definition o f infant mental health. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Mental Health Steering Committee.

Received January 1, 2014 Revision received April 23, 2014 Accepted April 27, 2014 ■

Correction to Brewster et al. (2014) In the article “Moving Beyond the Binary With Disordered Eating Research: A Test and Extension of Objectification Theory With Bisexual Women ” by Melanie E. Brewster, Brandon L. Velez, Jessica Esposito, Stephanie Wong, Elizabeth Geiger, and Brian TaeHyuk Keum (Journal o f Counseling Psychology, 2014, Vol. 61, No. 1, pp. 50-62. doi:10.1037/a0034748), the name of author Brian TaeHyuk Keum was misspelled as TaeHyuk Brian Keum. The online version of this article has been corrected. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038172

Copyright of Journal of Counseling Psychology is the property of American Psychological Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Neurosciences of infant mental health development: recent findings and implications for counseling psychology.

This article is about the neural correlates of infant mental health and their correspondences to social emotional development. These correspondences a...
6MB Sizes 3 Downloads 11 Views