C International Psychogeriatric Association 2014 International Psychogeriatrics (2014), 26:5, 759–767  doi:10.1017/S1041610214000027

Only social feedback reduces age-related prospective memory deficits in “Virtual Week” ...........................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

Agnieszka Nied´zwie´nska,1 Peter G. Rendell,2 Krystian Barzykowski1 and Alicja Leszczy´nska1 1 2

Applied Memory Research Laboratory, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland School of Psychology, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne, Australia

ABSTRACT

Background: Prospective memory, or remembering to do things in the future, is crucial for independent living in old age. Although there is evidence of substantial age-related deficits in memory for intentions, older adults have demonstrated the ability to compensate for their deficits in everyday life. The present study investigated feedback as a strategy for facilitating prospective memory in the elderly. Method: Young and older adults played a computer-based task, Virtual Week, in which they had to remember to carry out life-like intentions. After each virtual day, specific feedback on prospective memory performance was automatically provided on the computer screen that participants either proceeded through by themselves (non-social feedback) or were taken through by an experimenter (social feedback). The control group received no feedback. Results: We found that, compared with no-feedback group, only social feedback substantially reduced the age-related deficit in prospective memory. Older adults significantly benefited from feedback provided by the experimenter on the tasks of intermediate difficulty. Unexpectedly, prospective memory with non-social feedback was not only worse than with social feedback, but it was not any better than without any feedback at all. Conclusions: The results extended previous findings on the effectiveness of feedback in improving the memory performance of older adults to include memory for intentions. Despite the feedback meeting the critical recommendations of being specific, objective, and well-targeted, it was ineffective when the feedback displayed on the computer was not introduced by the experimenter. This has implications for computerized training tasks where automated feedback is considered crucial. Key words: aging, cognitive activity, memory

Introduction Prospective memory (PM) refers to memory for future intentions (e.g. taking medication, paying bills) and it is crucial for independent living in old age. Although there is evidence of substantial age-related deficits in PM (e.g. Henry et al., 2004; Knight et al., 2008), older adults have demonstrated the ability to compensate for their deficits in everyday life (Rendell and Craik, 2000; Schnitzspahn et al., 2011). The present study set out to identify effective strategies of facilitating PM in older adults. Specifically, we tested whether

´ Correspondence should be addressed to: Agnieszka Nied´zwienska, Institute of Psychology, Mickiewicza 3, 31-120 Kraków, Poland. Phone: +48-509-222953; Fax: +48-12-623-76-99. Email: [email protected]. Received 2 Sep 2013; revision requested 7 Oct 2013; revised version received 28 Nov 2013; accepted 23 Dec 2013. First published online 4 February 2014.

providing older adults with feedback would improve their PM performance. Feedback is defined as “post-response information that is provided to learners to inform them of their actual state of learning or performance” (Narciss, 2008, pp. 126). Considerable evidence has been gathered to suggest that feedback has a positive effect on learning and memory (e.g. Vollmeyer and Rheinberg, 2005; Metcalfe and Kornell, 2007; Wang and Wu, 2008), provided that certain recommendations regarding the form of feedback are followed (see Hattie and Timperley, 2007; Shute, 2008, for further reviews). A key theme in these recommendations is that the feedback should be objective, specific, presented in manageable units, and well-targeted. Research has shown that feedback can also improve memory performance in the elderly (West et al., 2005; Da Silva and Sunderland, 2010; Tse et al., 2010).

760

A. Nied´zwie´nska et al.

One could assume that feedback should be even more beneficial for older than younger adults as their performance is usually worse and there is more room for improvement. For instance, Gielen et al. (2010) demonstrated that participants with worse pretest performance benefited more from receiving feedback than participants who initially performed better. Da Silva and Sunderland (2010) actually demonstrated that older compared to young adults showed greater improvement with feedback. There are a number of different ways in which feedback can improve task performance (Hattie and Timperley, 2007; Shute, 2008). One group of mechanisms involves the use of cognitive processes, such as using new memorization strategies, correcting inappropriate strategies or some procedural errors. For PM tasks, memorization strategies may involve more elaborative encoding of intentions or more frequent rehearsals during the retention period. Another group of mechanisms is related to affective processes. According to Hattie and Timperley (2007) feedback can improve performance through affective processes by increasing engagement, effort, and motivation. The aging literature suggests that placing feedback in the social context may be an effective way of strengthening its affective influence in the elderly. The presence of other people generally tends to enhance positive emotions (see Fischer et al., 2003, for a review), but the elderly seem to be especially susceptible to the positive impact of social interactions. This relates to the social selectivity theory, that is supported by research showing older adults are generally more oriented towards socially meaningful interactions (Carstensen, 1992). Older adults also displayed stronger social commitment when completing a task (West et al., 2005). What is more, there is research that showed highlighting the social context of the PM task improved performance in older but not younger adults (Altgassen et al., 2010). Therefore, for older adults feedback presented during social interaction may be more meaningful and encouraging than feedback they proceed through by themselves. Given the effectiveness of feedback in facilitating learning and memory, we expected that feedback would improve PM performance. Using the results of Da Silva and Sunderland (2010) and Gielen et al., (2010) suggesting older adults and lower pretest performers benefiting most from feedback, we predicted that a positive effect of feedback would be more pronounced in older than young adults. On the basis of the social selectivity theory (Carstensen, 1992), we speculated that only older adults would benefit more from feedback presented

by an experimenter than from the same feedback they proceeded through by themselves.

Method Participants A total of 75 young adults (age: M = 21.64 years, SD = 2.10, range = 19–29) and 72 older adults (age: M = 70.36 years, SD = 5.66, range = 61–87) participated in the study. There is uneven number of participants in the age groups as 6 young and 9 older participants were not able to be included as their data was incomplete due to technical difficulties during the experimental session. The groups did not significantly differ in the relative frequencies of men and women (26.7% and 34.7% men for young and older adults respectively), χ2 (1) = 1.12, p = 0.289 or years of formal education, t(145) = 1.44, p = 0.152 (young adults: M = 14.81, SD = 1.89; older adults: M = 15.38, SD = 2.77). The older adult participants were screened for the presence of dementia, using the Polish version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 2009) (all participants ࣙ27). They were all physically active, healthy, and living independently. Two measures of cognitive ability were used to establish a neuropsychological baseline for all participants. Speed of processing was tested using the digit symbol subtest of the Polish version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale´ Revised (Brzezinski et al., 2004). Verbal ability was measured using the synonym subtest of the APISZ test, which is recognized as a reliable estimate of crystallized intelligence (Matczak et al., 2006). Young adults outperformed older adults in the test of processing speed, t(145) = 18.16, p < 0.001 (young adults: M = 69.33, SD = 11.20; older adults: M = 38.17, SD = 9.50), but they performed worse than older adults on verbal ability, t(145) = 3.04, p = 0.003 (young adults: M = 5.73, SD = 3.15; older adults: M = 7.56, SD = 4.08). Thus, this sample is representative of other aging studies. We randomly assigned 27 young and 27 older adults to each of the three feedback conditions: none, non-social, social. However, the number of participants included in the analyses for each age group for the three conditions was: young: 24, 24, 27; older: 25, 24, 23. The participants from each age group were evenly assigned to the three conditions but then 15 participants were not included due to technical difficulties during the experimental session (see above). The three conditions did not significantly differ in demographical characteristics such as the relative frequencies of men and women (young adults: χ2 (2) = 1.54; older adults: χ2 (2) = 0.04, ps ࣙ 0.463), age (Fs < 1 for young and

Social feedback and prospective memory

older adults) or years of formal education (young adults: F(2, 72) = 0.08; older adults: F(2, 69) = 1.53, ps ࣙ 0.225). Similarly, the conditions did not significantly differ in processing speed (Fs < 1 for young and older adults) or verbal ability (young adults: F(2, 72) = 1.10; older adults F(2, 69) = 0.71, ps ࣙ 0.338). Participants were told that the study would examine how people cope with different types of everyday tasks. They were not given any more information about the research goals and hypothesis until the study was over. Materials and procedure The experimenters conducting were co-authors and thus were not blind to the experiment but they did learn and follow closely very precise responses to specific scenarios. VIRTUAL WEEK

This is a computer-based laboratory measure of PM designed to represent PM in daily life (original version, Rendell and Craik, 2000). It uses a computerized board game format, in which participants move around the board with the roll of a die. Each circuit of the board represents a waking day. As participants move around the board, they must pick up event cards. There are 10 event cards per virtual day with each card presenting participants with a brief description of an activity relevant to the virtual time of day (e.g. a meal or shopping) and a decision to make with three options related to the daily activity (e.g. what to eat or what to buy). In addition, participants have to remember to carry out life-like intentions (PM tasks). The choosing of activities, rolling dice and moving the token constitute the ongoing activity for the PM tasks embedded in Virtual Week. Each day of Virtual Week included eight PM tasks (four regular, four irregular) and did not include the two-time check tasks, as in recent studies (e.g. Mioni et al., 2013). Participants did not physically carry out the PM tasks; rather they clicked on the “Perform Task” button when they felt it was the appropriate moment and selected the task from a list of possibilities (PM tasks and distracters). The four regular PM tasks were the same each day and simulated the kinds of regular tasks that occur as one undertakes normal duties (such as taking medication every day at the same time), two were event-based (triggered by specific event cards: breakfast and dinner event cards), and two were time-based (triggered by specific times on virtual time clock of day: 11am and 9pm). The timebased tasks required monitoring of a virtual clock in a prominent position. As in recent studies (e.g.

761

Mioni et al. 2013), this study used the computerized version of Virtual Week with a virtual time clock, rather than consecutive hours of day marked on squares on the board, as in original version. This virtual time clock was calibrated to the position of the token on the board. The four irregular PM tasks were different each day and simulated the kinds of occasional tasks which occur in everyday life (such as phone plumber and pick up dry-cleaning). Again, the four irregular tasks consisted of two event-based and two time-based tasks (e.g. pick up dry-cleaning when shopping triggered by event card depicting shopping; and phone plumber at 5 p.m. triggered by virtual clock). As observed in recent studies with Virtual Week (Mioni et al., 2013) the critical distinction is that regular relative to irregular PM tasks in Virtual Week impose substantially fewer demands on retrospective memory function. This is because they (i) are learnt to criterion as opposed to encountered incidentally; (ii) are repeatedly performed rather than one-off non-recurring tasks; (iii) involve fewer cue action associations across the duration of Virtual Week (4 vs. 20), and (iv) involve less complex content. The first virtual day was preceded by a trial day. The experimenter interacted with the participant during the trial day, talking them through the procedures, prompting and giving feedback with each PM response. In each condition, participants then completed five virtual days. NO FEEDBACK CONDITION

Participants played the standard version of Virtual Week. The only difference was that after each virtual day they were engaged in a filler task that lasted the same time period as it took to provide feedback in two other conditions. For the filler task, participants rated pictures of restaurants with the names of the restaurants printed on them. They rated how much they like the restaurant, how much they like the name of the restaurant, and to what extent the name fits the appearance of the restaurant. Participants were first given the filler task at the end of the trial day and then proceeded through it by themselves after each virtual day but with instructions on the computer screen to engage in filler and stop filler task. At the end of the trial day, participants were only told that they would be given an additional task after each virtual day and all instructions they would need to complete this task would be given on the computer screen. NON-SOCIAL FEEDBACK CONDITION

Virtual Week was modified to provide specific feedback on PM performance at the end of each virtual day, the trial day included. This was

762

A. Nied´zwie´nska et al.

provided via a sequence of three computer screen displays that participants proceeded through by themselves. This feedback was designed to meet the recommendations for feedback reported in the introduction: specific, objective, and well-targeted, and presented in manageable units. Separating the feedback into three screens ensured it was in manageable units. On the first screen display, participants were informed for each PM task separately whether their response was correct, miss, little late, lot late, little early or lot early. This feedback met the criteria of objective and specific feedback. The second display presented their overall performance during the day in a pie chart. The third display included a line chart that presented how they had performed on each day so far. The three screens presented the feedback at three distinct levels: the response on each specific task for the virtual day; the performance overall for the virtual day; and performance over all virtual days. The first screen display was presented for 40 seconds and the other two for 20 seconds each, with a 10 seconds gap before each feedback window when there was displayed a request to wait for data to be calculated. At the end of the trial day, this feedback was presented and discussed with participants. During the trial day participants were told that they would be given feedback on their performance after each virtual day and were asked to read it carefully. They were told that each feedback window would stay on for set time to ensure that they would have ample time to read and take in the feedback but the experimenter would not comment on or discuss the feedback with them. After the trial day and at the end of each virtual day, participants proceeded through the three feedback windows by themselves. The experimenter was always present in a room to monitor whether participants followed the instructions as indicated.

SOCIAL FEEDBACK CONDITION

In this condition, specific feedback on PM performance was also provided at the end of each virtual day, the trial day included, but participants were taken through the feedback by the experimenter. The same three screen displays as in the non-social feedback were presented for 40, 20, and 20 seconds respectively. However, each display was introduced by the experimenter during the 10 second interval before each feedback window. The experimenter said: “You have managed to reach the end of your virtual day. I will present you your results in a minute. Let’s have a look together how you coped with the tasks. Please read carefully the information you will be given.” The experimenter then referred to the specific form of feedback that

was going to be presented, e. g. “Let’s have a look at which tasks you managed to cope well with and which you did not.” or “Let’s have a look what your overall performance during the day was.” No more information was given either before or during the presentation. As in the non-social feedback condition, participants were told during the trial day that they would be given feedback on their performance and at the end of the trial day the feedback was presented and discussed with participants. However in this condition, they were also told the experimenter would go through the results with them after each virtual day. Participants were told that the experimenter wished to help and make the game more interesting for them, and to this purpose, would devote some time to present their results after each virtual day. However, it was stressed that the experimenter would not comment on or discuss the feedback during the presentation. Summing up, all participants received the same information about the aim of study and were identically taught how to play Virtual Week during the trial day. The difference between the conditions appeared for the first time at the end of the trial day when the instructions about feedback or the filler task were provided. In the non-social feedback condition participants were only told to read the feedback carefully. In the social feedback condition they were also told that the experimenter had prepared the feedback having their good in mind and would go through their results with them. Other differences between the two conditions appeared before the feedback presentation after each virtual day. In the non-social feedback condition each feedback window was preceded by a computer screen displaying that the results are being calculated, whereas in the social feedback condition it was introduced by the experimenter. However, in neither condition the feedback was explained or commented on. In the present study, reliability as indexed by Cronbach’s alpha for all PM tasks (40 PM tasks, 8 per virtual day) across all feedback conditions was .73 for young and .89 for older. Statistical analyses The statistical package SPSS-20 was used for data analysis. The overall PM performance as function of age group and feedback condition was analyzed with a 2 × 3 × 2 × 2 mixed ANOVA with between-group variables age group (young, older), feedback condition (none, non-social, social), and within-groups variables PM task (regular, irregular) and PM cue (event-based, time-based). This was followed up with separate factorial ANOVAs, with

Social feedback and prospective memory

the between-group variable of age group and feedback condition, for each of the four measures comprising the within groups measures of PM task and PM cue: regular event-based, regular time-based, irregular event-based, and irregular time-based. In all ANOVAs, interactions were analyzed with tests of simple effects. All post hoc tests for simple effects were conducted as pair-wise Bonferroni comparisons, adjusted for multiple comparisons. For simple effects, effect sizes were calculated as partial eta-squared but for post hoc Bonferroni paired comparison effect sizes were calculated as Cohen’s d.

Results A 2 × 3 × 2 × 2 mixed ANOVA on the proportion of correct PM tasks revealed main effects of age, F(1, 141) = 132.52, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.49, PM task, F(1, 141) = 82.62, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.37, and PM cue, F(1, 141) = 26.16, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.16, but not feedback, F(1, 141) = 1.03, p = 0.360, η p 2 = 0.01. There were several significant interaction effects and critically a four-way interaction, F(2, 141) = 3.25, p = 0.042, η p 2 = 0.04. As the primary interest was the effect of age group and feedback condition, this was followed up with separate analyses of these between-groups variables for regular event-based tasks, regular time-based tasks, irregular event-based tasks and irregular time-based tasks. The proportions correct on each of these tasks are displayed in Figure 1, as function of age group and feedback condition. Regular event-based PM Analysis of these tasks revealed a main effect of age group, F(1, 141) = 47.78, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.25, but not feedback condition, F(2, 141) = 0.53, p = 0.591, η p 2 < 0.01. There was no interaction effect F(2, 141) = 0.38, p = 0.684, η p 2 < 0.01. Figure 1 shows the consistent age-related deficits across all three feedback conditions. Regular time-based PM Analysis of these tasks revealed a main effect of age group, F(1, 141) = 60.41, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.30, and feedback condition while not a main effect F(2, 141) = 1.39, p = 0.253, η p 2 = 0.02 was an interaction effect F(2, 141) = 9.05, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.11. Follow-up simple effects analysis of the interaction revealed older were worse than young within no feedback, F(1, 141) = 22.10, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.14, and non-social feedback, F(1, 141) = 53.56, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.28, but young and older did not differ for social feedback, F(1, 141) =

763

1.94, p = 0.166, η p 2 = 0.01. Further analysis of the interaction revealed a simple main effect of feedback condition for older, F(2, 141) = 8.24, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.11, but not young adults, F(2, 141) = 1.98, p = 0.141, η p 2 = 0.03. For older adults Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed that participants with nonsocial feedback performed worse than both social feedback (p < 0.001, d = 1.05) and no feedback (p = 0.029, d = 0.51), and approaching significance (p = 0.057, d = 0.52) was worse performance on no feedback compared to social feedback. Irregular event-based PM Analysis of these tasks revealed a main effect of age group, F(1, 141) = 74.94, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.35, and feedback condition while not a main effect F(2, 141) = 1.27, p = 0.284, η p 2 = 0.02 was an interaction effect F(2, 141) = 3.09, p = 0.048, η p 2 = 0.04. Follow-up analysis of the interaction revealed a simple main effect of age group within each feedback condition: no feedback, F(1, 141) = 34.733, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.20; non-social feedback, F(1, 141) = 36.87, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.21; and social feedback, F(1, 141) = 9.02, p = 0.003, η p 2 = 0.06. Older adults were consistently worse on each condition and effect size was similar on no feedback and non-social feedback but much smaller on social feedback. Further analysis of the interaction revealed a simple main effect of feedback condition for older, F(2, 141) = 3.87 p = 0.023, η p 2 = 0.05, but not young adults, F(2, 141) = 0.32, p = 0.730, η p 2 < 0.01. For older adults, Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed, participants with social feedback performed better than both no feedback (p = 0.036, d = 0.51) and non-social feedback (p = .009, d = 0.67), but no feedback and non-social feedback did not differ (p = 0.579, d = 0.12). Irregular time-based PM Analysis revealed a main effect of age group, F(1, 141) = 227.83, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.63, and feedback condition while not a main effect F(2, 141) = 0.91, p = 0.405, η p 2 = 0.01 was an interaction effect F(2, 141) = 4.71, p = 0.011, η p 2 = 0.06. Follow-up analysis of the interaction revealed a simple main effect of age group within each feedback condition: no feedback, F(1, 141) = 82.67, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.37; non-social feedback, F(1, 141) = 111.89, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.44; and social feedback, F(1, 141) = 41.47, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.23. Older adults were consistently worse on each condition, with large effect sizes on each condition but both no feedback and non-social feedback had larger effects sizes than social feedback. Further analysis of the interaction revealed a simple main effect of feedback condition for older, F(2, 141) = 4.46 p = 0.013,

764

A. Nied´zwie´nska et al.

Figure 1. Mean proportion of correct PM tasks as a function of age group (young, old) and feedback condition (none, non-social, social). Error bars depict standard error of the mean.

η p 2 = 0.06, but not young adults, F(2, 141) = 1.03, p = 0.360, η p 2 = 0.01. For older adults, Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed participants with social feedback performed better than non-social feedback (p = 0.003, d = 0.78) but no feedback did not differ with both non-social feedback (p = 0.130, d = 0.41) nor social feedback (p = 0.133, d = 0.37).

Discussion The main finding was that older adults significantly benefited from feedback provided by the experimenter. Young adults performed equally well no matter whether they were provided with feedback or not. The positive effect of social feedback on the performance of older adults was

medium in magnitude for irregular event-based tasks and regular time-based tasks. Social feedback substantially reduced or eliminated age-related differences in PM performance on those tasks. It also reduced the age deficit on irregular time-based tasks to some extent. In light of the consistent very good performance by young adults, the claim that age-related deficits have been eliminated needs qualifying and any conclusions about differences between young and older adults in their sensitivity to social feedback are limited. However, it is worth noting that for young adults there was still some room for improvement. Their overall performance was near ceiling, but for irregular PM tasks their performance without any feedback ranged from a proportion of 0.70 correct to 1.00 correct, with only one participant having a maximum score of 1.00. Nevertheless, the clear

Social feedback and prospective memory

and critical finding is that the performance of older adults has been enhanced by feedback provided by the experimenter. As in other research in which Virtual Week has been used (Henry et al., 2012), older adults generally had more difficulties with irregular than regular intentions and more difficulties with time-based than event-based PM: with the best performance on regular event-based tasks and poorest performance on irregular time. The pattern of feedback effects suggests that older adults did not benefit from social feedback on the least demanding task (regular event), benefited only to a small extent on the most demanding task (irregular time) but benefited substantially on the tasks of intermediate difficulty (regular time and irregular event). It may be that they did not try as hard to improve their performance on PM tasks they perceived as relatively easy to realize (e.g. take medication after breakfast every day). On the other hand, even their greatest efforts may not have been enough to substantially improve their performance on the most demanding PM tasks. The pattern of results accords with the multiprocess model (McDaniel and Einstein, 2000) that emphasizes that PM tasks can vary in the extent that strategic processes are required compared to automatic processes. For some PM tasks to be completed successfully, one needs to strategically monitor the environment for the presence of the target event or strategically monitor the time. However, when the target event is salient and strongly associated with the intended action, encountering the target event relatively automatically brings to mind the intended action. In our study, the least demanding task (regular event-based) could be invoking automatic processes and low level demands on strategic processes, and thus the feedback increasing motivation and a more strategic approach is not going to be as helpful. The other tasks could be requiring some level of strategic processes, and thus increased motivation supports more effortful strategic processing and thus improves performance. However, when the PM task is most demanding in terms of strategic processing, then it is possibly too demanding for increased motivation to help. Unexpectedly, PM with non-social feedback was not only worse than with social feedback, but it was not any better than without any feedback at all. For regular time-based tasks, it was even worse than in the no-feedback condition. One possible explanation is that introducing the feedback in the social context made it more salient compared to the non-social feedback condition. Although in both conditions the feedback was presented for the same amount of time and was neither explained nor discussed, participants may have

765

paid less attention to the feedback when they proceeded through it by themselves and thus it was not helpful. However, the finding that PM with non-social feedback was significantly worse than without any feedback for one type of tasks (and numerically worse for all other types) suggests another explanation. It is worth noting that older adults generally performed poorly on Virtual Week and therefore feedback they received was most often negative. Feedback highlighting relatively poor performance may have evoked negative emotions (e.g. frustration, threatened self-esteem) that can inhibit learning processes and effective use of information (Kluger and DeNisi, 1996). Research has shown that negative feedback elicits negative affect and consequently does not improve or even decreases subsequent performance in low selfefficacious people (Shrauger and Sorman, 1977; Kernis et al., 1989). Since the elderly have increasing difficulties with memory, self-efficacy and little confidence in their ability to remember (West et al., 1996), our older adult participants might have experienced these adverse reactions. It can be argued that positive emotions induced by the social context of receiving feedback were helpful in overcoming negative emotional responses to negative feedback. Thus, presenting feedback by the experimenter may have offset the negative impact of the content of the feedback and enabled effective use of received information. However, further research is needed to tease out whether social feedback enhanced performance amongst older adults because of effects on motivation via social emotional functions or increasing saliency of the feedback. Despite the feedback meeting the critical recommendations of specific, objective and welltargeted, it was only effective when the feedback displayed on the computer was presented by the experimenter, i.e. feedback in a social context. Interestingly, the non-social feedback condition, resembled the format that occurs in computer games, particularly in computer based training exercises, with specific results on performance displayed on computer screen. Therefore the findings of this study suggest that computer games/tasks displaying feedback on screen may not be effective for older adults unless this feedback can have a social aspect such as an experimenter or trainer taking the game participant through the feedback presented on computer screen. Thus these findings have implications for training programs that often involve computer based tasks and automated feedback (Lustig et al., 2009). Further accentuating the importance of these implications, is that feedback is thought to be one of the critical features of effective training programs (Lustig et al.,

766

A. Nied´zwie´nska et al.

2009). Along with other features like considerable repetitions of the training task, increasing difficulty, and that difficulty being adaptive to performance of the individual undertaking training (Basak et al., 2008). Interestingly, there appears to be a lack of systematic investigation of feedback in cognitive training studies, along with the lack of previous studies investigating feedback with PM tasks (Green and Bavelier 2008). This study makes an original contribution showing promising benefits of feedback from automated feedback in computer based PM tasks, but it was limited to situation where this feedback was augmented by the presence of the experimenter guiding them through the feedback. There is need for further research to explore why the effects limited to situation where the experimenter was present during feedback and effects were also limited on the least demanding and most demanding tasks but substantial on tasks of intermediate difficulty.

Conflict of interest None.

Description of authors’ roles All four authors collaboratively designed the study. ´ A. Nied´zwienska formulated the research questions, developed the Polish version of Virtual Week, supervised the data collection and wrote the paper. P.G. Rendell modified Virtual Week to the procedure requirements, was responsible for carrying out the statistical analysis and participated in writing the paper. K. Barzykowski and A. ´ Leszczynska collected the data and assisted in writing the paper.

Acknowledgments This research was supported by an International Project grant from the Polish National Science Center: 2011/01/M/HS6/00681. In addition it was supported by a Discovery Project grant from the Australian Research Council.

References Altgassen, M., Kliegel, M., Brandimonte, M. and Filippello, P. (2010). Are older adults more social than younger adults? Social importance increases older adults’ prospective memory performance. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 17, 312–28. doi:10.1080/ 13825580903281308.

Basak, C., Boot, W. R., Voss, M. W. and Kramer, A. F. (2008). Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 23, 765–777. doi: 10.1037/a0013494. ´ Brzezinski, J., Gaul, M., Hornowska, E., Jaworowska, A., Machowski, A. and Zakrzewska, M. (2004). Skala Inteligencji Wechslera dla Dorosłych. Wersja Zrewidowana – renormalizacja 2004 – WAIS-R(PL). Warszawa, Poland: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych. Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 7, 331–338. doi:10.1037/ 0882-7974.7.3.331. Da Silva, L. and Sunderland, A. (2010). Effects of immediate feedback and errorless learning on recognition memory processing in young and older adults. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 20, 42–58. doi:10.1080/09602010903036731. Fischer, A. H., Manstead, A. S. R. and Zaalberg, R. (2003). Social influences on the emotion process. European Review of Social Psychology, 14, 171–201. doi:10.1080/10463280340000054. Folstein, M., Folstein, S. and Fanjiang, G. (2009). Krótka Skala Oceny Stanu Umysłowego (MMSE). Warszawa, Poland: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych. Gielen, S., Peeters, E., Dochy, F., Onghene, P. and Struyven, K. (2010). Improving the effectiveness of peer feedback for learning. Learning and Instruction, 20, 304–315. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.08.007. Green, C. S. and Bavelier, D. (2008). Exercising your brain: a review of human brain plasticity and training-induced learning. Psychology and Aging, 23, 692–701. doi:10.1037/a0014345. Hattie, J. and Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Review of Educational Research, 77, 81–112. doi:10.3102/003465430298487. Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Philips, L. H. and Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. Psychology and Aging, 19, 27–39. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.27. Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., Phillips, L. H., Dunlop, L. and Kliegel, M. (2012). Prospective memory reminders: a laboratory investigation of initiation source and age effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1274–1287. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.651091. Kernis, M. H., Brockner, J. and Frankel, B. S. (1989). Self-esteem and reactions to failure: the mediating role of overgeneralization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 707–714. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.4. 707. Kluger, A. N. and DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feedback interventions on performance: a historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 254–284. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.254. Knight, R. G., Nicholls, J. and Titov, N. (2008). The effects of old age and distraction on the assessment of prospective memory in a simulated naturalistic environment. International Psychogeriatrics, 20, 124–134. doi:10.1017/S1041610207005923. Lustig, C., Shah, P., Seidler, R. and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2009). Aging, training, and the brain: a review and

Social feedback and prospective memory future directions. Neuropsychology Review, 19, 504–522. doi:10.1007/s11065-009-9119-9. Matczak, A., Jaworowska, A., Ciechanowicz, A. and ´ Stanczak, J. (2006). Bateria testów APIS-Z. Podr˛ecznik (Wyd. 2). Warszawa, Poland: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych. McDaniel, M. A. and Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S127–S144. doi:10.1002/acp.775. Metcalfe, J. and Kornell, N. (2007). Principles of cognitive science in education: the effects of generation, errors, and feedback. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 225–229. doi:10.3758/BF03194056. Mioni, G., Rendell, P. G., Henry, J. D., Cantagallo, A. and Stablum, F. (2013). An investigation of prospective memory function in people with traumatic brain injury using Virtual Week. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35, 617–630. doi:10.1080/ 13803395.2013.804036. Narciss, S. (2008). Feedback strategies for interactive learning tasks. In J. M. Spector, M. D. Merrill, J. J. G. van Merrienboer and M. P. Driscoll (eds.), Handbook of Research on Educational Communications and Technology, 3rd edn (pp. 125–144). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Rendell, P. G. and Craik, F. I. M. (2000). Virtual and Actual Week: age-related differences in prospective memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology. Special Issue: New Perspectives in Prospective Memory, 14, 43–62. doi:10.1002/acp.770. Schnitzspahn, K. M., Ihle, A., Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G. and Kliegel, M. (2011). The age-prospective

767

memory-paradox: An exploration of possible mechanisms. International Psychogeriatrics, 23, 583–592. doi:10.1017/S1041610210001651. Shrauger, J. and Sorman, P. B. (1977). Self-evaluations, initial success and failure, and improvement as determinants of persistence. Journal of Consulting And Clinical Psychology, 45, 784–795. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.45.5.784. Shute, V. J. (2008). Focus on formative feedback. Review of Educational Research, 78, 153–189. doi:10.3102/0034654307313795. Tse, C. S., Balota, D. A. and Roediger, H. L. (2010). The benefits and costs of repeated testing on the learning of face-name pairs in healthy older adults. Psychology and Aging, 25, 833–45. doi:10.1037/a0019933. Vollmeyer, R. and Rheinberg, F. (2005). A surprising effect of feedback on learning. Learning and Instruction, 15, 589–602. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2005.08.001. Wang, S.-L. and Wu, P.-Y. (2008). The role of feedback and self-efficacy on web-based learning: The social cognitive perspective. Computers and Education, 51, 1589–1598. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2008.03. 004. West, R. L., Dennehy-Basile, D. and Norris, M. P. (1996). Memory self-evaluation: The effects of age and experience. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 3, 67–83. doi:10.1080/13825589608256613. West, R. L., Bagwell, D. K. and Dark-Freudeman, A. (2005). Memory and goal setting: the response of older and younger adults to positive and objective feedback. Psychology and Aging, 20, 195–201. doi:10.1037/ 0882-7974.20.2.195.

Only social feedback reduces age-related prospective memory deficits in "Virtual Week".

Prospective memory, or remembering to do things in the future, is crucial for independent living in old age. Although there is evidence of substantial...
213KB Sizes 1 Downloads 0 Views