Overweight Adolescents and Life Events in Childhood Julie C. Lumeng, Kristen Wendorf, Megan H. Pesch, Danielle P. Appugliese, Niko Kaciroti, Robert F. Corwyn and Robert H. Bradley Pediatrics; originally published online November 11, 2013; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2013-1111

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the World Wide Web at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2013/11/06/peds.2013-1111

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2013 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Dalhousie Univ on November 14, 2013

Overweight Adolescents and Life Events in Childhood WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Psychosocial stress in childhood has been associated with a greater risk of future overweight, although the associations have not always been consistent, the types of psychosocial stressors have often been somewhat extreme, and moderators of the association have rarely been examined. WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Experiencing many negative life events in childhood, particularly with chronicity or events that are family health related, increases risk of overweight by age 15 years. Maternal obesity and greater delay of gratification for food each intensify this risk.

aDepartment of Pediatrics, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor, Michigan; bDepartment of Environmental Health Sciences, University of Michigan School of Public Health, Ann Arbor, Michigan; cCenter for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan; dDepartment of Pediatrics, University of California San Francisco School of Medicine, San Francisco, California; eData Coordinating Center, Boston University School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts; fDepartment of Psychology, University of Arkansas at Little Rock, Little Rock, Arkansas; and gFamily and Human Dynamics Research Institute, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona

KEY WORDS adolescence, overweight, psychosocial, stress

abstract OBJECTIVES: To test the association of life events in childhood with overweight risk in adolescence; to examine the effects of chronicity, timing, intensity, valence, and type of life events; and to test potential moderators. METHODS: Mothers of children enrolled in the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development responded to the Life Experiences Survey at ages 4, 9, and 11 years. Using logistic regression analysis, we tested the association of experiencing many negative life events with being overweight at age 15 years, controlling for child gender, race/ethnicity, maternal education, and maternal obesity. Child gender, maternal education, maternal obesity, child’s ability to delay gratification for food, and maternal sensitivity were tested as moderators. RESULTS: Among the 848 study children (82% non-Hispanic white), experiencing many negative life events was associated with a higher risk of overweight (odds ratio: 1.47 [95% confidence interval: 1.04–2.10]). Greater chronicity and negative valence of the event were associated with greater overweight risk; timing of exposure and maternal reported impact of the event were not. The association was more robust for events related to family physical or mental health and among children of obese mothers and children who waited longer for food. CONCLUSIONS: Children who experience many negative life events are at higher risk of being overweight by age 15 years. Future work should investigate mechanisms involved in this association, particularly those connected to appetitive drive and self-regulation; these mechanisms may hold promise for obesity prevention strategies. Pediatrics 2013;132:e1506–e1512

e1506

AUTHORS: Julie C. Lumeng, MD,a,b,c Kristen Wendorf, MD, MS,d Megan H. Pesch, MD, MS,a Danielle P. Appugliese, MPH,e Niko Kaciroti, PhD,c Robert F. Corwyn, PhD,f and Robert H. Bradley, PhDg

ABBREVIATIONS CI—confidence interval OR—odds ratio Dr Lumeng conceptualized and designed the study and drafted the initial manuscript; Drs Wendorf and Pesch assisted in conceptualizing and designing the study, contributed to drafting portions of the initial manuscript, and critically reviewed the manuscript; Ms Appugliese conducted statistical analyses and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Kaciroti directed statistical analyses and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Corwyn assisted in conceptualizing the study, assisted with data access and conceptualization of study variables, critically reviewed statistical analyses, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Bradley assisted in conceptualizing and designing the study, critically reviewed the statistical analyses, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted. www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2013-1111 doi:10.1542/peds.2013-1111 Accepted for publication Sep 23, 2013 Address correspondence to Julie C. Lumeng, MD, Center for Human Growth and Development, 300 North Ingalls St, 10th Floor, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-5406. E-mail: [email protected] PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275). Copyright © 2013 by the American Academy of Pediatrics FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose. FUNDING: Supported by an American Heart Association Midwest Affiliate Grant-in-Aid (0750206Z) to Dr Lumeng. POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

LUMENG et al

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Dalhousie Univ on November 14, 2013

ARTICLE

Psychological and physiologic adaptation to chronic stress, or allostatic load,1 has important implications for health and disease. The association between psychosocial stressors and risk of childhood obesity has been documented in several studies,2–8 although reports have provided less robust support for the association than anticipated,9 had null findings,10–13 or shown evidence of moderation according to age,9 gender,4,5 or other factors.11,12 Improved understanding of the association between psychosocial stress and childhood obesity would inform interventions. The definition of psychosocial stressors in previous studies has varied, with most studies combining various stressors into risk indices that include parental mental3,4,8,11,12 and physical8,11,12 health, child violence exposure,3–6,12 socioeconomic stressors,4,6,8,9,11,12 and nonoptimal family structure and functioning.6,8,11,12 Another approach involves asking parents to report events that occurred and to rate their impact. A checklist of events allows the inclusion of common events, and the parent (as opposed to the researcher) assigns the event a negative, positive, or neutral valence. Only 3 studies have used this approach, and the association of life events with the child’s obesity risk was inconsistent.7,10,13 The current study had 3 objectives: (1) to test the association of life events at ages 4, 9, and 11 years with overweight at age 15 years among US children of diverse socioeconomic status; (2) to examine the effects of chronicity, timing, intensity, valence, and type of event on overweight; and (3) to test moderation by factors linked with increased appetitive drive (ie, the desire to eat)14 or diminished self-regulatory capacity (ie, the ability to restrict intake).15 We specifically hypothesized that the child being less able to delay gratification for food, or having an obese or less sensitive mother, represented biological

and environmental factors that could intensify any effect of experiencing negative life events on overweight.

METHODS Sample We used data from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development, a longitudinal cohort of 1364 families from 10 US sites commencing at the time of the child’s birth in 1991 (https:// secc.rti.org). This study was approved by the institutional review boards of all participating institutions. Anthropometricdataatage15yearswere available for 864 children, of whom 863 had at least 1 life event measure, and 848 had complete data for gender, race/ ethnicity, maternal education, and maternal weight status. The 848 participants (82% non-Hispanic white) included in the analyses, compared with the excluded, incomplete sample, were less likely to be male (49.5% vs 50.5%; P = .04) and had mothers with more education (14.43 years vs 13.91 years; P , .001). There was no difference in race/ethnicity. Main Predictor: Life Events Mothers completed an adapted version of the Life Experiences Survey16 when the child had a mean 6 SD age of 4.7 6 0.1, 9.0 6 0.3, and 11.0 6 0.3 years. Mothers reported whether each of 71 events had occurred in the last year, and if it occurred, rated the impact on a 7-point Likert scale, from –3 (extremely negative) to 0 (no impact) to 3 (extremely positive). Number of negative life events was defined as the sum of all events endorsed that were also rated as having a negative impact (–1 to –3). The number of negative life events at each age was then averaged across ages. We categorized mean number of negative life events as “many,” defined as the highest quartile (range: 4.5–17.3 mean

number of negative life events across the 3 age points) versus “not,” defined as the lowest 3 quartiles (range: 0–4.3). We created a chronicity variable, defined as the number of age points a child was in the top quartile: never (54.0%), once (27.2%), and at 2 (13.8%) or 3 (5.0%) years of age. We created a timing variable to reflect the ages at which the child was in the top quartile: early only (in the top quartile at only age 4 years or only ages 4 and 9 years; n = 102), late only (in the top quartile at only age 11 years or only ages 9 and 11 years; n = 147), and never (n = 458). Children in the top quartile at all 3 ages (n = 42) or intermittently (top quartile only at age 9 years, or top quartile at ages 4 and 11 years but not age 9 years; n = 99) were excluded from the timing analysis. Impact ratings (–1 to –3) were reverse coded, summed at each age (range: 1– 182 [age 4 years], 1–47 [age 9 years], and 1–45 [age 11 years]) and averaged across ages to create an impact of negative life events variable. Total number of life events, regardless of whether the impact rating was positive, negative, or neutral, was summed at each age and then averaged across ages, with the top quartile defined as “many life events.” We created 4 categories of negative life events according to author consensus: parent or family physical or mental health and well-being (ie, family health; 15–18 items, depending on age [eg, “serious illness or injury of close family member,” “took prescription drugs for at least 1 month to help with mental problems”]; parental work, school, or financial stability (ie, family finances; 15 items [eg, “foreclosure on mortgage or loan,” “fired or laid off from job,” “return to work”); emotional aspects of relationships (ie, family relationships; 14 items [eg, “trouble with in-laws,” “argument or conflict with husband/ partner,” “divorce”); and family structure, routine, and caregiving (10 items; eg, “birth or adoption of a child,” “child

PEDIATRICS Volume 132, Number 6, December 2013

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Dalhousie Univ on November 14, 2013

e1507

started school,” “change in child custody arrangements,” “need to care for aging family member”). The number of events in each category was averaged across ages, and the top quartile for each category was defined as “many.” Outcome: Overweight at Age 15 Years Height and weight were measured by using a standardized protocol at age 15 years. Overweight was defined as a BMI $85th percentile for age and gender based on the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention growth charts.17 Covariates Child gender, race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic white versus not), and maternal education (continuous [in years]) were included as covariates. The data set includes only maternal BMI by selfreport when the child is age 15 years. Mothers’ images on videotape at child ages 15, 24, and 36 months were assigned a weight status ranging from 1 (thinnest) to 9 (heaviest) by using a standard pictorial rating scale,18 an approach with demonstrated validity,19 and ratings were then averaged. The mean maternal weight status rating correlated with maternal BMI when the child was aged 15 years (r = 0.74). Delay of gratification at age 4 years was assessed by using the Mischel and Ebbesen self-imposed waiting task.20 The child was told that the examiner was going to leave the room, and if the child could wait until the examiner returned, he or she would be allowed to eat a large quantity of a preferred food (candy, animal crackers, or pretzels). If the child could not wait, he or she would be allowed to eat only a small quantity. Children who waited were considered to have delayed gratification. Parenting sensitivity was defined as the mean sensitivity z score coded from videotaped structured mother–child play interactions21,22 at ages 15, 24, and 36 months.

e1508

Statistical Analysis

RESULTS

Statistical analysis was conducted by using SAS version 9.2 (SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC). For the 9.7% of the sample that was missing the number of negative life events (total or in each category), impact rating score, or total number of life events at 1 or 2 of the 3 age points, these values were imputed. Descriptive statistics were used to characterize the sample, and x 2 and t tests were used to evaluate the association of each covariate with overweight at age 15 years. We used multiple logistic regression analysis to evaluate the association of mean negative life events in childhood with being overweight at age 15 years, controlling for child race/ethnicity, child gender, maternal education, and maternal weight status. We next reran this model using as the predictor our created categorical variable “many negative life events.” Four new models were then retested, rerunning this fully adjusted model and replacing the many negative life events variable individually with each of the following variables: (1) chronicity of negative life events; (2) timing of negative life events; (3) impact of negative life events; and (4) many life events (of positive, negative, or neutral valence). To test the independent effect of each type of negative life event, we included the 4 different types of negative life events (family health, family finances, family relationships, and family structure, routine, and caregiving), each coded categorically as “many” versus “not” simultaneously in the model.

Table 1 displays characteristics of the sample and bivariate associations with overweight. The main effect of each life event variable in the fully adjusted model is shown in Table 2. The mean number of negative life events experienced in childhood independently predicted a higher risk of overweight in adolescence (OR: 1.08 [95% CI: 1.01– 1.14]). Experiencing many negative life events also independently predicted a higher risk of overweight (OR: 1.47 [95% CI: 1.04–2.10]).

Four additional models were tested, individually including interactions of the categorical many negative life events variable with child gender, maternal education, maternal weight status, the child’s ability to delay gratification, and parenting sensitivity. Adjusted odds ratios (ORs) and their corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated from all models.

Chronicity was important; experiencing many negative life events at $2 time points compared with never experiencing such events was associated with a higher risk of overweight (OR: 1.55 [95% CI: 1.03–2.34]); experiencing many negative life events just once was only marginally associated with a higher risk of overweight (OR: 1.37 [95% CI: 0.95– 1.98]). Experiencing many negative life events at $2 time points was not associated with a higher risk of overweight than experiencing many life events at just 1 time point (OR: 1.14 [95% CI: 0.72–1.78]). With regard to timing of experiencing many negative life events, although there was a marginal effect of early only versus never, as well as late only versus never, there was no main effect of early only versus late only (OR: 1.11 [95% CI: 0.63–1.96]). The impact of the negative life events was not associated with risk of overweight (OR: 1.00 [95% CI: 0.99–1.02]). The total number of life events (positive, negative, or neutral impact) was not associated with a higher risk of overweight (OR: 1.36 [95% CI: 0.95–1.94]). When different types of negative life events were included simultaneously in the model, only family health remained a significant predictor of overweight (OR: 1.81 [95% CI: 1.21–2.72]). The interactions of many negative life events with child gender and maternal education were not significant (P = .38

LUMENG et al

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Dalhousie Univ on November 14, 2013

ARTICLE

TABLE 1 Characteristics of the Sample According to Overweight Status at Age 15 Years Variable

Not Overweight (n = 588) Overweight (n = 260) Total (N = 848)

Male White race/ethnicity Mother’s education, y Maternal weight status No. of negative life events Many negative life events Chronicity of many negative life events Never At 1 age point At $2 age points Timing of many negative life events (n = 707) Early only Late only Never Impact of negative life events (n = 793) Many total life events Types of negative life events (% in highest quartile) Family health Family finances Family relationships Family structure, routines, and caregiving Unable to delay gratification for food (n = 741) Maternal parenting sensitivity

45.8 84.5 14.74 6 2.41 4.27 6 1.28 3.16 6 2.50 22.1

57.7 75.4 13.74 6 2.23 5.18 6 1.50 3.60 6 2.60 30.4

49.4 81.7 14.43 6 2.40 4.55 6 1.41 3.30 6 2.54 24.7

57.0 26.2 16.8

47.3 29.6 23.1

54.0 27.2 18.8

P .001 .002 ,.001 ,.001 .02 .01 .02

.04 12.7 19.8 67.5 6.29 6 10.20

18.5 23.2 58.3 7.00 6 4.89

14.4 20.8 64.8 6.51 6 8.90

.19

21.1

30.4

23.9

.003

17.5 26.4 20.8 24.3

24.6 33.9 22.3 33.1

19.7 28.7 21.2 27.0

.02 .03 .61 .01

41.7

56.9

46.2

,.001

0.13 6 0.76

20.20 6 0.81

0.03 6 0.79

,.001

Data are presented as % or mean 6 SD.

TABLE 2 Main Effects of Life Event Variables Predicting Overweight in Separate, Fully Adjusted Models (N = 848) Main Effect

OR (95% CI)

Negative life events Many negative life events versus not Chronicity of many negative life events At 1 age point versus never At $2 age points versus never Timing of many negative life events (n = 707) Early only versus never Late only versus never Impact of negative life events Many total life events versus not Types of negative life events Many family health events versus not Many family finances events versus not Many family relationship events versus not Many family structure, routines, and caregiving events versus not

1.08 (1.01–1.14) 1.47 (1.04–2.10) 1.37 (0.95–1.98) 1.55 (1.03–2.34) 1.61 (0.99–2.61) 1.44 (0.94–2.22) 1.00 (0.99–1.02) 1.36 (0.95–1.94) 1.81 (1.21–2.72) 1.35 (0.94–1.96) 0.71 (0.46–1.09) 1.35 (0.91–1.98)

Each life event variable main effect shown is adjusted for child gender, race/ethnicity, maternal education, and maternal weight status.

and P = .81, respectively). The interaction of maternal weight status with many negative life events was suggestive (P = .12). Among children of mothers with a figure rating $5 (a value sensitive and specific for obesity

in previous work19) (n = 309), the association of negative life events with overweight was significant (OR: 2.02 [95% CI: 1.25–3.52]). Among children of mothers with a figure rating ,5 (ie, nonobese) (n = 539), the association of

negative life events with overweight was not significant (OR: 1.15 [95% CI: 0.70–1.88]). The interaction of the child’s ability to delay gratification and negative life events was also suggestive (P = .07). Among children who delayed gratification (n = 399), the association of negative life events with overweight was significant (OR: 1.96 [95% CI: 1.14 – 3.3]). Among children who did not delay gratification, the association of negative life events with overweight was not significant (OR: 1.02 [95% CI: 0.59–1.76]). The interaction of parenting sensitivity with negative life events was not significant (P = .31).

DISCUSSION Experiencing many negative life events during childhood increased the risk of overweight at age 15 years independent of child gender, race/ethnicity, maternal education, and mother’s weight status in these study subjects. The more chronic the exposure, the stronger the effect, although the timing of the exposure did not matter. Motherreported intensity of negative impact was not associated with overweight risk. The valence of the life event mattered: events perceived as negative were associated with increased overweight risk, but total events were not. The type of negative event also mattered: events related to family health were most robustly associated with adolescent overweight, independent of whether there were also many life events related to family finances, relationships, structure, routines, and caregiving. The effect of negative life events was equally robust in both boys and girls and among children of mothers with varying levels of education. The association was more robust among children of mothers with a higher weight status as well as among children who waited for a larger amount of food at age 4 years.

PEDIATRICS Volume 132, Number 6, December 2013

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Dalhousie Univ on November 14, 2013

e1509

Our finding that chronicity of negative life events is an important predictor of overweight risk requires replication, particularly because 2 previous studies have reported conflicting results, finding greater chronicity associated with both higher5 and lower4 overweight risk. Our finding that the association did not differ based on the timing of exposure to negative life events is similar to most4,7,8 (but not all9,11) previous studies, suggesting that there does not seem to be a developmental window in childhood during which the association of negative life events or stressors with overweight is more robust. Our finding that the maternalreported impact of negative life events was not associated with overweight risk may be consistent with a previous study demonstrating that the objective report of stressors in the home was predictive of child obesity, whereas the parent’s self-reported perceived stress was not.8 These results suggest that negative life events may have an impact on overweight risk even if they are not perceived as intensely stressful by the parent. This finding may occur either because the parent does not recognize the effect of the life events on the child or because the life events affect the child’s stress physiology but not the observable behavior.23 To our knowledge, our findings that the valence of events is important, and that some types of negative life events are more robustly associated with adolescent overweight, have not previously been reported. We did not detect moderation according to gender, unlike 2 previous studies that examined moderation and found more robust associations in girls.4,5 Given that eating in response to stress is reported more commonly in women,24,25 potential gender differences deserve additional consideration. We did not detect moderation according to maternal education, consistent with the e1510

existing literature in which the association has been identified in samples of varying and diverse socioeconomic status.3–8 The finding that the association of negative life events with overweight was more robust among children of obese mothers differs from the single previous study examining this question.7 Obese mothers may have more obesitypromoting home environments, which could potentiate the effect of negative life events. They may also share the same neurobiology with their child that promotes excessive eating in response to stressors (eg, hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenal axis functioning,14 differences in neural circuitry related to food reward,26 genetic variants associated with eating behaviors [including both emotional eating and cognitive restraint]27). The finding that the association was more robustamong childrenwho waited fora largerquantity of food was contrary to our expectations, as well as contrary to a recent study which found that the ability to delay gratification mediated the association of psychosocial risk with child BMI.28 We hypothesize that the child’s delay of gratification for food may reflect not just a general capacity for self-regulation but also how much the child desires the food or finds the food to be a motivator. Several studies have found that the reduced ability to delay gratification for food,29–31 as well as for nonfood items (toys),29 is associated with more rapid rates of weight gain and future obesity risk. This classic behavioral task using food may be tapping into more than just self-regulatory capacity but also interindividual differences in children’s motivation to attain a larger quantity of a palatable food or sensitivity to reward.32 The effect of negative life events on the development of overweight may occur through increased appetitive drive, as opposed to reduced self-regulatory capacity. In

addition, not all children who experience significant stress respond with greater impulsivity; rather, some use more positive coping strategies.33 Understanding these pathways should be a focus of future work. There are several mechanisms through which negative life events may be associated with future overweight. Negative life events may lead to increased food consumption,34–38 particularly comfort foods, which contribute to relieving stress at a neurobiological level.14 Negative life events may also lead to poor sleep or disruption of household routines, which have been linked to increased obesity risk.39,40 Negative life events may also contribute to child behavioral or mental health problems, which have each been linked to childhood obesity.41,42 Previous work has also shown that under conditions of stress, the brain triggers changes in metabolism that can lead to excessive weight gain.43 Strengths of our study include its relatively large, prospective, and socioeconomically and geographically diverse sample. Of the studies that measured negative life events as we did,7,10,13 our study included the most detailed measure. Limitations include that our sample was primarily white. Our study, like most previous work,4,5,7–13 asked mothers to describe events or stressors in the home but did not ask children directly about their experience of these events or stressors.

CONCLUSIONS Early school-aged children experiencing many negative life events are at higher risk of being overweight by age 15 years. At present, clinicians might consider attending to the possibility of increases in overweight risk among children experiencing a substantial number of negative life events concurrently at any point in childhood, as well as children living in households in

LUMENG et al

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Dalhousie Univ on November 14, 2013

ARTICLE

which adults or family members are experiencing life events related to their own physical or mental health and wellbeing. As the mechanism becomes

better understood, interventions could be developed to better target the mechanism of association to prevent obesity. Investment in parenting and

early childhood education programs, which can mitigate early life adversity,44 may be a valuable focus for policies targeting childhood obesity.

low-income US children: results from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (1999-2002). Pediatrics. 2008;122(3). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/122/3/e529 Lohman BJ, Stewart S, Gundersen C, Garasky S, Eisenmann JC. Adolescent overweight and obesity: links to food insecurity and individual, maternal, and family stressors. J Adolesc Health. 2009;45(3):230–237 Moens E, Braet C, Bosmans G, Rosseel Y. Unfavourable family characteristics and their associations with childhood obesity: a cross-sectional study. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2009;17(4):315–323 Dallman MF, Pecoraro NC, la Fleur SE. Chronic stress and comfort foods: selfmedication and abdominal obesity. Brain Behav Immun. 2005;19(4):275–280 Arnsten AF. Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(6):410– 422 Sarason IG, Johnson JH, Siegel JM. Assessing the impact of life changes: development of the Life Experiences Survey. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1978;46(5):932–946 Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Grummer-Strawn LM, et al. CDC growth charts: United States. Adv Data. 2000;314(314):1–27 Stunkard AJ, Sorensen TI, Schulsinger F. Use of the Danish Adoption Register for the Study of Obesity and Thinness. New York, NY: Raven Press; 1983 Cardinal TM, Kaciroti N, Lumeng JC. The figure rating scale as an index of weight status of women on videotape. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2006;14(12):2132–2135 Mischel W, Ebbesen EB. Attention in delay of gratification. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1970;16 (2):329–337 United States Department of Health and Human Services. National Institutes of Health. Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development: Phase I, 1991–1995 [United States]. Quality of maternal care: rating of mother in a structured play situation; 24-month home visit, child care data

addendum-78a. ICPSR21940-v1. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor], 2010-0108. doi:10.3886/ICPSR21940.v1 United States Department of Health and Human Services. National Institutes of Health. Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development: Phase I, 1991–1995 [United States]. Quality of maternal care and child behaviors with mother: rating of other and child in a structured play situation, 36-month home visit form, child data report 136. ICPSR21940-v1. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor], 2010-0108. doi:10.3886/ICPSR21940.v1 Bauer AM, Quas JA, Boyce WT. Associations between physiological reactivity and children’s behavior: advantages of a multisystem approach. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2002;23 (2):102–113 Korkeila MK, Kaprio J, Rissanen A, Koshenvuo M, Sörensen TI. Predictors of major weight gain in adult Finns: stress, life satisfaction and personality traits. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1998;22(10):949–957 DiPietro LAR, Anda RF, Williamson DF, Stunkard AJ. Depressive symptoms and weight change in a national cohort of adults. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1992; 16(10):745–753 Stice E, Figlewicz DP, Gosnell BA, Levine AS, Pratt WE. The contribution of brain reward circuits to the obesity epidemic. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;10(12):00210–00212 Cornelis MC, Rimm EB, Curhan GC, et al. Obesity susceptibility loci and uncontrolled eating, emotional eating and cognitive restraint behaviors in men and women [published online ahead of print August 8, 2013]. Obesity (Silver Spring). doi: 10.1002/ oby.20592 Evans GW, Fuller-Rowell TE, Doan SN. Childhood cumulative risk and obesity: the mediating role of self-regulatory ability. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1). Available at: www. pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/129/1/e68 Francis LA, Susman EJ. Self-regulation and rapid weight gain in children from age 3 to

REFERENCES 1. McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1998;840:33–44 2. Gundersen C, Mahatmya D, Garasky S, Lohman B. Linking psychosocial stressors and childhood obesity. Obes Rev. 2011;12 (5):e54–e63 3. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14(4):245–258 4. Suglia SF, Duarte CS, Chambers EC, BoyntonJarrett R. Cumulative social risk and obesity in early childhood. Pediatrics. 2012;129(5). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/129/5/e1173 5. Boynton-Jarrett R, Fargnoli J, Suglia SF, Zuckerman B, Wright RJ. Association between maternal intimate partner violence and incident obesity in preschool-aged children: results from the Fragile Families and Child Well-being Study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(6):540–546 6. Wells NM, Evans GW, Beavis A, Ong AD. Early childhood poverty, cumulative risk exposure, and body mass index trajectories through young adulthood. Am J Public Health. 2010;100(12):2507–2512 7. Koch FS, Sepa A, Ludvigsson J. Psychological stress and obesity. J Pediatr. 2008;153 (6):839–844 8. Parks EP, Kumanyika S, Moore RH, Stettler N, Wrotniak BH, Kazak A. Influence of stress in parents on child obesity and related behaviors. Pediatrics. 2012;130(5). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/ full/130/5/e1096 9. Garasky S, Stewart SD, Gundersen C, Lohman BJ, Eisenmann JC. Family stressors and child obesity. Soc Sci Res. 2009;38(4): 755–766 10. Gibson LY, Byrne SM, Davis EA, Blair E, Jacoby P, Zubrick SR. The role of family and maternal factors in childhood obesity. Med J Aust. 2007;186(11):591–595 11. Gundersen C, Lohman BJ, Garasky S, Stewart S, Eisenmann J. Food security, maternal stressors, and overweight among

12.

13.

14.

15.

16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

21.

22.

23.

24.

25.

26.

27.

28.

29.

PEDIATRICS Volume 132, Number 6, December 2013

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Dalhousie Univ on November 14, 2013

e1511

30.

31.

32.

33.

34.

12 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009; 163(4):297–302 Schlam TR, Wilson NL, Shoda Y, Mischel W, Ayduk O. Preschoolers’ delay of gratification predicts their body mass 30 years later. J Pediatr. 2013;162(1):90–93 Seeyave DM, Coleman S, Appugliese D, et al. Ability to delay gratification at age 4 years and risk of overweight at age 11 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(4):303– 308 Davis C, Strachan S, Berkson M. Sensitivity to reward: implications for overeating and overweight. Appetite. 2004;42(2):131–138 Dise-Lewis JE. The life events and coping inventory: an assessment of stress in children. Psychosom Med. 1988;50(5):484– 499 Ganley R. Emotion and eating in obesity: a review of the literature. Int J Eat Disord. 1989;8(3):343–361

e1512

35. Nguyen-Rodriguez ST, Chou CP, Unger JB, Spruijt-Metz D. BMI as a moderator of perceived stress and emotional eating in adolescents. Eat Behav. 2008;9(2):238–246 36. Canetti L, Bachar E, Berry EM. Food and emotion. Behav Processes. 2002;60(2):157– 164 37. Pecoraro N, Reyes F, Gomez F, Bhargava A, Dallman MF. Chronic stress promotes palatable feeding, which reduces signs of stress: feedforward and feedback effects of chronic stress. Endocrinology. 2004;145(8): 3754–3762 38. Danese A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease: depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(12):1135–1143 39. Lumeng JC, Somashekar D, Appugliese D, Kaciroti N, Corwyn RF, Bradley RH. Shorter

40.

41.

42.

43.

44.

sleep duration is associated with increased risk for being overweight at ages 9 to 12 years. Pediatrics. 2007;120(5):1020–1029 Anderson SE, Whitaker RC. Household routines and obesity in US preschool-aged children. Pediatrics. 2010;125(3):420–428 Lumeng JC, Gannon K, Cabral HJ, Frank DA, Zuckerman B. Association between clinically meaningful behavior problems and overweight in children. Pediatrics. 2003;112 (5):1138–1145 Mustillo S, Worthman C, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Angold A, Costello EJ. Obesity and psychiatric disorder: developmental trajectories. Pediatrics. 2003;111(4 pt 1):851–859 Hitze B, Hubold C, van Dyken R, et al. How the selfish brain organizes its supply and demand. Front Neuroenergetics. 2010;2(7):7 Barnett WS. Effectiveness of early educational intervention. Science. 2011;333 (6045):975–978

LUMENG et al

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Dalhousie Univ on November 14, 2013

Overweight Adolescents and Life Events in Childhood Julie C. Lumeng, Kristen Wendorf, Megan H. Pesch, Danielle P. Appugliese, Niko Kaciroti, Robert F. Corwyn and Robert H. Bradley Pediatrics; originally published online November 11, 2013; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2013-1111 Updated Information & Services

including high resolution figures, can be found at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2013/11/06 /peds.2013-1111

Permissions & Licensing

Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or in its entirety can be found online at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xh tml

Reprints

Information about ordering reprints can be found online: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2013 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Dalhousie Univ on November 14, 2013

Overweight adolescents and life events in childhood.

To test the association of life events in childhood with overweight risk in adolescence; to examine the effects of chronicity, timing, intensity, vale...
650KB Sizes 0 Downloads 0 Views