This article was downloaded by: [Massachusetts PRIM Board] On: 08 April 2015, At: 14:43 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Psychology & Health Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gpsh20

Perseverative thoughts and subjective health complaints in adolescence: Mediating effects of perceived stress and negative affects a

ab

c

Gyöngyi Kökönyei , Anna Józan , Antony Morgan , Eszter ab

a

bd

Szemenyei , Róbert Urbán , Melinda Reinhardt

& Zsolt

a

Demetrovics a

Institute of Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary b

Click for updates

Doctoral School of Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary c

School of Health and Life Sciences, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK d

Institute of Psychology, Károli Gáspár University of the Reformed Church, Budapest, Hungary Accepted author version posted online: 20 Jan 2015.Published online: 09 Feb 2015.

To cite this article: Gyöngyi Kökönyei, Anna Józan, Antony Morgan, Eszter Szemenyei, Róbert Urbán, Melinda Reinhardt & Zsolt Demetrovics (2015): Perseverative thoughts and subjective health complaints in adolescence: Mediating effects of perceived stress and negative affects, Psychology & Health, DOI: 10.1080/08870446.2015.1007982 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2015.1007982

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or

howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/termsand-conditions

Psychology & Health, 2015 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2015.1007982

Perseverative thoughts and subjective health complaints in adolescence: Mediating effects of perceived stress and negative affects Gyöngyi Kökönyeia*, Anna Józana,b, Antony Morganc, Eszter Szemenyeia,b, Róbert Urbána, Melinda Reinhardtb,d and Zsolt Demetrovicsa

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

a

Institute of Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary; bDoctoral School of Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary; cSchool of Health and Life Sciences, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK; dInstitute of Psychology, Károli Gáspár University of the Reformed Church, Budapest, Hungary (Received 16 July 2014; accepted 8 January 2015) Stable tendency to perseverative thoughts such as trait rumination and worry can influence somatic health. The purpose of the study was to investigate the relationship between perseverative thoughts and somatic complaints, and the possible mediating effects of perceived stress, negative and positive affectivity in adolescence. Having an acute or a chronic condition was also assessed to be controlled for and to reveal their effects on symptom reporting. Three hundred and six adolescents from 7th to 12th grade with mean age of 16.33 (SD = 1.29) participated in the study. Mediation analysis suggested that impact of trait-like perseverative thoughts on complaints were mediated by perceived stress and negative affectivity. Having an acute condition had also an effect on symptom reporting through increased negative affectivity. Our results highlight that ruminations or worry as stable intrapersonal characteristics are relevant processes in health and can be potential targets in prevention programmes in adolescence. Keywords: perseverative thoughts; subjective health complaints; perceived stress; negative affectivity; acute condition

Introduction Subjective health complaints (SHC) refer to a set of somatic and psychological symptoms experienced by the individual with or without a defined diagnosis (Eriksen & Ursin, 2004). Both somatic complaints such as fatigue (Jellesma, Rieffe, & Terwogt, 2007), headache, abdominal pain (Saps et al., 2009) and psychological symptoms such as irritability or nervousness (Hagquist & Andrich, 2004) are common even in otherwise healthy adolescents. Co-occurrence of symptoms is not rare, and warrants special attention, because it is associated with poor general health, low life satisfaction (Ravens-Sieberer et al., 2009) and more medicine used (Gobina et al., 2011). SHC have been theorised to be at least partly related to psychosocial stress. Frequent or sustained stress and the related emotional and physiological arousal may *Corresponding author. Email: [email protected] © 2015 Taylor & Francis

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

2

G. Kökönyei et al.

have an etiological role in the development of frequent complaints (Brosschot, 2002; Dantzer, 2005; Ursin & Eriksen, 2004). During adolescence, school and peer-related stressors are reliable predictors of SHC. For example, failures in school (Hurrelmann, Engel, Holler, & Nordlohne, 1988), lack of support from teachers (Ghandour, Overpeck, Huang, Kogan, & Scheidt, 2004) being bullied or non-acceptance by peers (Jellesma, Rieffe, Terwogt, & Kneepkens, 2006; Natvig, Albrektsen, Anderssen, & Qvarnstrom, 1999; Nixon, Linkie, Coleman, & Fitch, 2011), all have a direct effect on the frequency of complaints. Family related stress, such as communication problems (Moreno et al., 2009) and conflicts with parents (Hurrelmann et al., 1988) are also strongly correlated with symptoms. However, previous studies rarely provide information about the mechanisms of symptom perception and symptom reporting. According to the perseverative cognition hypothesis, it is not stress itself or reactivity to stressors that are the most predictive of chronic SHC. Instead it is the prolonged consequence (such as prolonged cardiovascular activity) that remain beyond the presence of the stressor that are more potent factors for predicting somatic health (Brosschot, 2002; Brosschot, Gerin, & Thayer, 2006; Brosschot, Pieper, & Thayer, 2005). According to this perspective, stressful events can only lead to SHC if repeated or sustained stress-related cognition prolongs the physiological stress-related activity (Brosschot et al., 2006). Perseverative cognitions such as rumination, worry or anticipated stress are capable of maintaining stress-related physiological and psychological arousal, and thus contribute to the development of SHC (Brosschot et al., 2005, 2006). Stress-related prolonged cardiovascular reactivity and cortisol response have been considered to be the physiological pathways that might mediate between perseverative thoughts and somatic health. In a recent review Zoccola and Dickerson (2012) concluded that in experimental laboratory studies stress-related state rumination measures were associated with greater cortisol response to the stressor. A daily life study applying ecological momentary assessment has revealed that mean daily ruminative self-focus predicted higher cortisol level (Huffziger et al., 2013). After cognitive stress delayed heart rate recovery has been found among high-trait worriers (Verkuil, Brosschot, de Beurs, & Thayer, 2009), and delayed blood pressure has been detected if the person ruminated about their task after finishing it (Radstaak, Geurts, Brosschot, Cillessen, & Kompier, 2011). Studies using momentary assessments show increased cardiovascular reactivity in worry episodes (Brosschot, Van Dijk, & Thayer, 2007; Pieper, Brosschot, van der Leeden, & Thayer, 2010), and worry episodes had an effect on heart rate and heart rate variability 2 h later (Pieper et al., 2010) or even predicted lowered heart rate variability during the subsequent night (Brosschot et al., 2007), all providing ecologically valid evidences for the relationship between perseverative thoughts and cardiovascular health. The latter results also indicate that worry episodes have prolonged effect on physiological functioning. Adaptation (habituation) to repeated stressors can decrease the allostatic load on the body (McEwen, 1998), and some recent studies have suggested that perseverative cognitions after stressors prevent this adaptive response. Post-stress rumination, for example, predicted non-habituation of HPA-axis (cortisol) response to the repeated Trier Social Stress Test on a subsequent day (Gianferante et al., 2014), and ruminators failed to adapt their cardiac responses (blood pressure and heart rate) to a repeated 5-min emotional recall task (Johnson, Lavoie, Bacon, Carlson, & Campbell, 2012). These results might implicate that not only prolonged activation of stress-related responses but

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

Psychology & Health

3

the lack of habituation to repeated stressors also contribute to the adverse effect of rumination on somatic health. Perseverative cognitions such as rumination or worry are cognitive factors in psychological symptoms or disorders (e.g. in depression or anxiety) both in adults (Borkovec, 1994; Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008) and in adolescents (e.g. Legerstee, Garnefski, Verhulst, & Utens, 2011). Some studies have provided support for the relationship between perseverative cognition and somatic health complaints in adults (Borkovec, 1994), in adolescents (Brosschot & van der Doef, 2006; Lohaus et al., 2013) and in children (Jellesma, Rieffe, & Terwogt, 2006). Rumination and worry represent a recurrent negative thinking about events that happened in the past, or may happen in the future, respectively (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Both rumination and worry are self-focused thoughts on negative and/or threatening materials or events (McLaughlin, Borkovec, & Sibrava, 2007) and are difficult to control (Ehring & Watkins, 2008). Furthermore, concerning problem-solving, perseverative thinking seems to be unproductive (Lyubomirsky, Tucker, Caldwell, & Berg, 1999). People differ in the extent they tend to engage in perseverative thoughts (trait-like perseverative thinking). People who are high on trait rumination tend to focus on negative aspect of self or events frequently, especially in times of sad moods (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991) or in case of failures (Martin, Tesser, & McIntosh, 1993). Nevertheless, perseverative thoughts can be induced by certain instructions in experimental studies (McLaughlin et al., 2007). Both trait-like perseverative thinking and induced perseverative thinking are associated with increased occurrence of negative affects (NA) and decreased occurrence of positive affects (PA) (McLaughlin et al., 2007). Similarly, real life rumination assessed by dairies or experience sampling is associated with increased NA and decreased PA (Brans, Koval, Verduyn, Lim, & Kuppens, 2013; Genet & Siemer, 2012). According to both the experimental and real-life studies, rumination precedes the development of increased NA. Similarly, the stress reactivity model of rumination (Alloy et al., 2000) emphasises that rumination makes the perceived stress more intense, disturbing and uncontrollable which in turn increases NAs. Sustained psychological arousal is accompanied with physiological arousal that may be perceived as somatic symptoms. Namely, Eriksen and Ursin (2004) suggest that SHC are based on sensations associated with normal physiological processes such as sympathetic activation during stress. For some individuals, these ‘normal’ sensations (such as butterflies in the stomach during stress) capture attention, and they might perceive and report them as complaints (stomach ache) if the stress is prolonged. According to the results of a prospective study, these non-productive perseverative cognitions (e.g. worry and rumination) preceded the development of complaints, but complaints did not cause perseverative cognition in adolescence (Jellesma et al., 2006). The primary aim of this study was to test the relationship between perseverative thoughts and somatic complaints in adolescents. Based on previous theoretical and empirical work described above, the perceived stress, NA and PA were expected to be mediators between perseverative thoughts and complaints. This study also hypothesised that perceived stress would increase NA and decrease PA (Figure 1). The existence of a chronic condition might have an effect on the symptoms experienced, because physical well-being of a child with a chronic condition is usually lower when compared to that of a healthy peer (Tzavara et al., 2012). Furthermore, having a chronic condition might have an impact even on the stress experienced (see Sentenac et al., 2012). Similarly, an

4

G. Kökönyei et al. Negative Affects

Perseverative thoughts

Perceived stress

Somatic complaints

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

Positive Affects

Figure 1. The theoretical model of the mediating effect of perceived stress, negative and PA between trait perseverative thoughts and somatic complaints.

acute condition, e.g. a viral infection, is also associated with certain physical symptoms (including fatigue, headache, hyperalgesia) and with psychological symptoms such as depressive mood (Dantzer, Bluthe, Laye, et al., 1998; Maier & Watkins, 1998). Having an acute condition may influence symptom reporting if the time interval assessing symptoms is short (e.g. one or two weeks). Therefore, we included questions about chronic and acute conditions, and controlled for their effects in the analyses. Methods Participants and procedure Three hundred and six adolescents (134 boys and 172 girls) between the ages of 13 and 18; from 7th to 12th grade with mean age of 16.33 (SD = 1.29) participated in the study. The adolescents were invited from nine secondary grammar schools (using a convenience sampling method), the response rate are above 90% (94.5%). Twenty-three per cent of the participants lived in the capital, 53% had their residence in other town and 24% of the respondents lived in villages. The majority of the participants (63.7%) reported that their mothers having college or university degree, 22% reported secondary school education of their mothers, while 10.5% reported lower education, and some students (3.6%) did not know their mothers’ highest education. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Eötvös Loránd University. Informed consent was received from parents prior to the implementation of the study. All adolescents answered the questionnaires in their own classrooms. Measures Non-productive Thought Questionnaire for Kids (Jellesma, Meerum Terwogt, Reijntjes, Rieffe, & Stegge, 2005). The scale contains 10 items that capture trait-like perseverative thoughts – such as ‘I often worry’ or ‘When I have a problem, I cannot stop thinking about it’. The items are

Psychology & Health

5

answered on a three-point scale (0 = not true, 1 = sometimes true, 2 = often true). The internal consistency of the scale was good (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.80) similarly to earlier results (Garnefski, Koopman, Kraaij, & ten Cate, 2009; Jellesma, Verkuil, & Brosschot, 2009; Miers, Rieffe, Terwogt, Cowan, & Linden, 2007). The validity of the scale was supported with positive correlations with other maladaptive emotion regulation scales, such as catastrophising (Garnefski, Rieffe, Jellesma, Terwogt, & Kraaij, 2007).

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

Positive and Negative Affectivity Schedule (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) The PANAS includes ten positive (e.g. proud, interested) and ten negative (e.g. nervous, distressed) affects rated on a five-point Likert-type scale (from 1 = very slightly or not at all to 5 = very much) and covered a 2-week period in our study. Both the negative affectivity and positive affectivity subscales had good internal consistency with an earlier study (Gyollai, Simor, Koteles, & Demetrovics, 2011) and in this study (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.84 and 0.83, respectively). Somatic Complaints List (Jellesma et al., 2007) SCL is an 11-item scale measuring the occurrence of somatic complaints. Two items of 11 are reverse scored and their content refers to being healthy. Participants were asked to indicate on a five-point Likert-type scale how often the items apply to themselves, with 1 = almost never to 5 = quite often. In our study SCL covered the last two weeks, and similarly to earlier results (Jellesma, Rieffe, Terwogt, & Westenberg, 2008, 2011) the internal consistency of the scale was good (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.81). Perceived Stress Scale-4 (PSS-4, Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983) We used a four item measure to assess subjective stress for the last month (e.g. ‘In the past month, how often have you felt that difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them?’). Each item was rated on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from never (0) to almost always (4). The Hungarian version has good psychometric properties (Stauder & Konkoly Thege, 2006) and in our study the reliability of the scale was good (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.74). Data regarding acute conditions in the last two weeks (e.g. flu, viral or bacterial infection) and chronic conditions requiring regular medical investigation (e.g. asthma, heart-related problems) were collected. Seventy-nine students (25.8%) reported having one or more acute condition in the last two weeks, and 40 students reported having a chronic condition (13.1%). Chronic conditions such as asthma, allergy, heart diseases and kidney diseases were reported. Statistical analysis SPSS 19.0 and Mplus 6.01 statistical software packages were used for statistical analyses. The proposed mediation model (see Figure 1) was tested with path analysis within structural equation modelling (SEM). Due to sampling method (class as a unit) and deviation from normal distribution, in SEM analysis maximum likelihood estimation robust to non-normality (MLR) was used (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2007) and cluster

6

G. Kökönyei et al.

effect was controlled. To evaluate the overall model fit, absolute fit index (chi-square value), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis fit index or non-normed fit index (TLI or NNFI) and root mean square error approximation (RMSEA) were inspected. CFI and TLI are related to the total variance accounted by the model, values higher than 0.95 indicate a good-fit. RMSEA is related to the variance of residuals, values below 0.05 indicate a good-fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993).

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

Results Descriptive analyses were carried out and gender differences were examined (Table 1). Statistically significant differences were found across all scales. Girls had higher scores than boys on the scales measuring non-productive thoughts, somatic complaints, negative affectivity and perceived stress, but had lower scores on positive affectivity. The effect sizes (Cohen d) were small for positive affectivity, and medium for all other scales (Table 1). Age was associated positively with non-productive thoughts, complaints, perceived stress and negative affectivity significantly (Table 2). Having an acute condition and having a chronic condition are linked with more complaints, as we expected (see Table 2). Adolescents with an acute condition in the last two weeks scored higher on both the perseverative thoughts and negative affectivity scales (Table 2). We hypothesised that the relationship between perseverative thoughts and complaints would be mediated by negative and PA and perceived stress (see Figure 1). The degree of fit of the proposed mediational model was tested by the means of SEM. An initial model was created controlling for gender, age, acute and chronic condition. The absolute and relative fit indices indicated only an acceptable fit (χ2 = 35.433, df = 12, p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.081 [0.051–0.113], CFI = 0.947, TLI = 0.884). Further examination of modification indices showed that having an acute condition in the last two weeks had an effect on NA. As a result we modified our model (see Figure 2), and after that all the indices indicated a good-fit (χ2 = 14.974, df = 11, p = 0.184; RMSEA = 0.035 [0.000–0.075], CFI = 0.991, TLI = 0.979). The final model (see Figure 2) showed that perseverative thoughts had no direct effect on complaints, and on PA, but did have a direct effect on perceived stress and NA. Perceived stress had a significant direct effect on both negative and PA and on

Table 1. Mean, standard deviations (SD) and effect sizes (Cohen d) by gender. Mean (SD) (total sample) Perseverative thoughts (NPTQ) Somatic complaints (SCL) Positive affects (PA) Negative affects (NA) Perceived stress (PSS-4)

Cohen d

Boys

Girls

t/d

9.23 (4.21)

7.53 (3.77)

10.54 (4.07)

6.58***

0.77

21.19 (6.36) 33.23 (4.21) 19.98 (6.65) 5.97 (2.86)

19.34 (6.08) 34.31 (6.61) 18.37 (6.01) 5.11 (2.44)

22.59 (6.22) 32.41 (7.32) 21.19 (6.87) 6.63 (2.99)

4.56*** 2.31** 3.67*** 4.85***

0.53 0.27 0.44 0.56

Notes: NPTQ: non-productive thought questionnaire, SCL: somatic complaints list, PA: positive affects, NA: negative affects, PSS-4: perceived stress scale. **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Psychology & Health

7

Table 2. Scale, age (acute and chronic) condition correlation. 2

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

1. Perseverative thoughts (NPTQ) 2. Somatic complaints (SCL) 3. Positive affects (PA) 4. Negative affects (NA) 5. Perceived stress (PSS-4)

3

4

5

Age

Acute cond.+

.33*** −.26***

.50***

.58***

.16** −.14*

−.47***

.57***

.52***

.15*

−.23***

−.23*** −.47*** −.11 −.04 .60*** .20** −.25*** .18** −.06

Chronic cond.++ .07 .12* −.06 .05 .08

Notes: NPTQ: non-productive thought questionnaire, SCL: somatic complaints list, PA: positive affects, NA: negative affects, PSS-4: perceived stress scale. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; +point biserial correlation, (1): had acute condition, (2): did not have acute condition; ++point biserial correlation, (1): has a chronic condition, (2): does not have a chronic condition; The coefficients with *** are significant according to Bonferroni correction (p < 0.002).

Negative Affects

0.20

0.07

0.47

Perseverative thoughts

0.57

Perceived stress

0.23

-0.21

0.41

Acute condition

-0.11

Somatic complaints

-0.49 -0.08

0.04 Positive Affects

-0.10

Figure 2. The final mediation model and standardised path coefficients. Notes: Dash arrows: non-significant path coefficients. Double arrow: covariance between the errors of negative and positive affectivity measures. Acute condition is coded: (1): having any acute condition, (2): not having acute condition.

somatic complaints. While NA had a significant effect on complaints, PA lacked this association with complaints. No significant direct path associate was found between acute condition and complaints. However, acute condition had a significant direct effect on NA. Examination of possible indirect effects showed four mediated pathways to be significant: ‘perseverative thoughts→NA→Complaints’ (standardised indirect effect was 0.08, p < 0.001), ‘perseverative thoughts→perceived stress→NA→Complaints’ (standardised indirect effect was 0.11, p < 0.001), and ‘perseverative thoughts→perceived stress→complaints’ (standardised indirect effect was 0.08, p < 0.001), and ‘acute condition→NA→complaints (standardised indirect effect was −0.09, p < 0.001).

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

8

G. Kökönyei et al.

Therefore, perseverative thoughts had an effect via both perceived stress and via NA on complaints, and via perceived stress→NA pathway. Pathways from perseverative thoughts to complaints via PA were non-significant partly due to lack of significant relationship between PA and complaints. The final model explained 41% of the total variance of somatic complaints among adolescents. Since our study has a cross-sectional design, multiple equivalent/competing alternative models can be produced. In our model (see Figure 1) NA explains the variance of complaints. Though we would have formulated our theoretical model differently in which complaints explained the variance of NA. If the relationship between complaints and NA is simply reversed (SHC→NA), all the indices indicated good-fit as the fit indices of our original model did: χ2 = 15.588, df = 11, p = 0.157; RMSEA = 0.037 [0.000–0.077], CFI = 0.990, TLI = 0.975. Similarly, relationship between perseverative thoughts and NA can be reversed or even bidirectional (Brans et al., 2013; Moberly & Watkins, 2008a). If we reversed the relationship between PC and NA (NA→Perseverative thoughts), all the fit statistics indicated a poor-fit: χ2 = 65.092, df = 15, p = 0.000; RMSEA = 0.105 [0.080–0.132], CFI = 0.897, TLI = 0.794. Discussion In our cross-sectional study perseverative thoughts were connected to SHC, and this association was fully mediated by NA and perceived stress. Our results therefore indicate that rumination exerts its effects on SHC not directly, but through perceived stress and negative affectivity. We argue that rumination represents a cognitive-emotional sensitisation process (Brosschot, 2002), because it is associated with higher perceived stress and increased NA. Those who are prone to ruminating or worrying elongate their state of stress resulting in their recovery psychologically and physiologically being hampered or delayed (O’Connor, Walker, Hendrickx, Talbot, & Schaefer, 2013; McCullough, Orsulak, Brandon, & Akers, 2007; Zoccola & Dickerson, 2012; Zoccola, Figueroa, Rabideau, Woody, & Benencia, 2014). Unlike other studies (Pressman & Cohen, 2005) no evidence was found between the occurrence of frequent somatic complaints and the protective role of PA. Our cross-sectional study did find, however, experiencing NA had a much stronger effect on complaints than experiencing PA. This is contrast to (Cohen, Alper, Doyle, Treanor, & Turner, 2006; Cohen, Doyle, Turner, Alper, & Skoner, 2003) experimental studies and Pettit, Kline, Gencoz, Gencoz, and Joiner (2001) prospective study. Though the relationship between PA and physical health is inconsistent as Pressman and Cohen’s (2005) review summarised. For example, in a recent cross-sectional study found a positive relationship between trait PA and physical health (Nath & Pradhan, 2012) but in a prospective study with undergraduates found that state PA did not predict physical health, while physical health predicted subsequent PA (Finch, Baranik, Liu, & West, 2012). It is worth noting that in our cross-sectional study state positive affectivity was assessed instead of trait one and the PANAS we used did not differentiate between selfgenerated and passively experienced PA. Some recent longitudinal field experiment found that self-generate positive emotions were associated with less SHC (Fredrickson,

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

Psychology & Health

9

Cohn, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008) and increased vagal tone as an index of objective marker of physical health (Kok et al., 2013). Self-reported NA had a direct effect on complaints, and mediated the relationship between perseverative thoughts and complaints. It is worth mentioning that this result indirectly contradicts classic alexithymia theory (see Nemiah, 1996; Sifneos, 1973; Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1991), which emphasised that unawareness of emotion was a potent factor in developing somatic complaints or even somatic diseases and conceptualised alexithymia as a tendency to express psychological distress in somatic rather than emotional way. Though we could not exclude the possibility that deficits in identifying emotions as a key aspects of alexithymia might contribute to the development of SHC (De Gucht & Heiser, 2003) via undifferentiated emotional responses (Taylor et al., 1991) or via increased physiological stress responses (Friedlander, Lumley, Farchione, & Doyal, 1997; Wehmer, Brejnak, Lumley, & Stettner, 1995). Furthermore, alexithymia has been demonstrated to be related to maladaptive (abstract-analytical) rumination (Di Schiena, Luminet, & Philippot, 2011). Further studies need to explore the possible relationship between perseverative thoughts and alexithymia and reveal whether they exert their effects on somatic or even on psychological symptoms through distinct pathways. In our study, perceived stress had a negative effect on PA. According to the dynamic model of affect (Zautra, Smith, Affleck, & Tennen, 2001) the relative independence of positive and negative affect is context-dependent. During calm states or times without stress amount of NA and PA can be independent, but during stress this affective complexity – described as a relative independence of NA and PA – may decrease, resulting increased NA and decreased PA. Our results concord with previous research which has shown that the prevalence of somatic and psychological symptoms increases with age, and girls report more complaints than boys (Hagquist, 2009). In our study, girls were also found to report more frequent perseverative thoughts, negative affect and evaluated perceived stress higher than boys, while boys reported more PA in the last two weeks than girls. Gender differences in cognitive vulnerabilities such as rumination are well-documented in the literature (Johnson & Whisman, 2013) and thought to contribute the well-known gender differences in mood (Weissman et al., 1993) and anxiety disorders (McLean, Asnaani, Litz, & Hofmann, 2011). Our study highlights that gender differences in perseverative thoughts may contribute the well-known gender differences in syndromes characterised by medically unexplained symptoms (Kroenke & Spitzer, 1998). Perseverative thoughts – such as worry and rumination – have been consistently linked to depressive and/or anxious symptoms or psychopathology in adults (see Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008) and in adolescents (e.g. Calvete, Orue, & Hankin, 2013; Michl, McLaughlin, Shepherd, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2013) as well. There are small numbers of studies that have investigated the effect of perseverative thoughts on somatic health during adolescence. Our study demonstrated that individual differences in perseverative thoughts were important in explaining the variance of health complaints among school-aged youth and pointed out that trait perseverative thoughts made the perceived stress stronger. During adolescence social approval is a major concern, and one may argue that stressors relating to social evaluative threat might be more frequent and salient. Occurrence of the stressors is not totally under the control of the individual, but response to them could be modified, as a simple worry intervention demonstrated: postponing worry to a daily 30- min worry episode reduced health

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

10

G. Kökönyei et al.

complaints (Brosschot & van der Doef, 2006; Jellesma et al., 2009). Large-scale international studies on youth’s health should consider including variables that might mediate or even moderate the relationship between stressors (e.g. bullying) and complaints. Perseverative thoughts are, therefore, warranted when investigating the relationship between stressors and health (Verkuil, Brosschot, Gebhardt, & Thayer, 2010). Having an acute condition was found to have an effect on symptom reporting. Those who had experienced an acute condition reported more symptoms, and according to the path analysis this was due to the increased NA. The body’s first response to infection has been called acute phase response or ‘sickness’ characterised by complex physiological and behavioural adjustment mediated by innate or non-specific immunity (Dantzer, Bluthe, Laye, et al., 1998). Acute viral illnesses are accompanied by non-specific symptoms, just like fever, somnolence, hyperalgesia, headache, fatigue, nausea, depressed mood and are attributable to cytokine production in central nervous system during infection (Chrousos, 2000; Dantzer, Bluthe, Gheusi, et al., 1998; Maier & Watkins, 1998). The list of symptoms typically contains items about headache, fatigue, pain in arms and legs, dizziness, shaky and shivery. If someone has just recovered from a viral infection, she/he will report frequent complaints for the last weeks. Our study therefore suggests that assessing acute conditions is important to take account of the fact that people are more likely to report symptoms following an acute episode. Based on earlier studies investigation well-being of adolescents with chronic condition (e.g. Tzavara et al., 2012), it was hypothesised that having a chronic condition would have an effect on symptom reporting. We did not find a relationship between chronic condition and rumination, though only a minority of students reported having a chronic condition. Other studies investigating relationship between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and emotional health have reported that rumination seem to be an important factor in depressive symptoms (Kraaij & Garnefski, 2012) and in lower level of health-related quality of life (Garnefski et al., 2009). In our study the proportion of adolescents reporting chronic condition was 13%, which is very similar to that found in a national representative sample (14.9%, Költő & Kökönyei, 2011). In our study, only a weak association between chronic condition and symptoms was found. If a list of symptoms captures a longer period, e.g. last six months, perhaps enquiring about chronic condition is advisable, though according to our results having a chronic condition did not affect the reported negative and positive affectivity and perceived stress. Limitations This study has some limitations. The cross-sectional study design used does not allow us to exclude the possibilities that perceived stress, NA and complaints had an effect on perseverative thought. However, a previous prospective study supports the findings that the relationship between rumination and symptoms was unidirectional (Jellesma et al., 2006). Only self-report measures were used in our study that could have resulted in an overestimation of the effects detected. Besides subjective perceived stress objective physiological indices (such as heart rate or heart rate variability) could have strengthened our results. For the other variables, other measures would not really have been better as internal states are subjective. Another limitation is that the measure of perseverative thinking did not distinguish between the two facets: rumination and worry. Though the two concepts have overlapping features, they might be uniquely associated

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

Psychology & Health

11

with somatic symptoms, as has been demonstrated in relation to depressive and anxious symptoms and coping behaviour in adolescents (Hong, 2007). Retrieving a past or imagining a possible future event characterised by negative affectivity should have been differentiated from the way of thinking about or analysing them (Kross, Gard, Deldin, Clifton, & Ayduk, 2012). Both rumination and worry refer to a special way of dealing with materials associated with NA. In this study we treated them as unproductive and maladaptive processes, though certain type of rumination called self-reflection or self-distancing could be fruitful to elaborate events with negative emotions (Kross, Ayduk, & Mischel, 2005; Kross, Duckworth, Ayduk, Tsukayama, & Mischel, 2011). Further studies are needed to explore the possible role of adaptive and maladaptive self-reflection in developing SHC. One could argue that no perseverative thoughts that lead to higher perceived stress, but it is perceived stress that induces that kind of thinking. We cannot exclude this possibility, but in our study trait-like perseverative thoughts were measured, that explains the direction of process illustrated in the path analysis. Similarly, relationship between perseverative thoughts and NA can be reversed or even bidirectional, as it has been demonstrated by momentary assessment studies (Brans et al., 2013; Moberly & Watkins, 2008a). Therefore, we tested the reversed relationship between PC and NA (NA→Perseverative thoughts), but all the fit statistics of this alternative model indicated a poor-fit, though this result might be due to our cross-sectional design. However, when not just rumination but other regulation strategies (e.g. reappraisal, distraction, social sharing, suppression) are measured simultaneously with experience sampling method those who experienced NA applied different strategies – e.g. suppression, distraction, rumination, reappraisal – to regulate negative feelings (Brans et al., 2013). This result shows that when experiencing NA a person is likely to use different strategies, so NA → rumination is not a specific relationship. It is worth noting that in a momentary study, Moberly and Watkins (2008b) found that trait ruminators reported greater negative affect after a negative event which is in line with our model that suggests that those who are tend to use perseverative thoughts will report increased level of NA. From a theoretical point of view, symptom perception model (Watson & Pennebaker, 1989) states that both trait and state negative affectivity or any general psychological distress lead to symptom perception through increased self-focus and attentional and interpretational biases. Concerning the relationship between (state) negative affectivity and complaints, therefore, we hypothesised that NA would explain the variance of SHC. Though we would have formulated our theoretical model differently in which complaints explained the variance of NA. For instance, in a study with a short prospective daily diary design, there was a cross-lagged association between state NA and pain symptoms among adults (Charles & Almeida, 2006). In line with their results our alternative model proved to be as good as our suggested model. From a statistical point of view, producing and comparing equivalent cross-sectional SEM models has both the advantage and disadvantage. SEM approach allows the test of competing equivalent models but there are some drawbacks of producing many equivalents models as well. If more than one model is supported by fit indices, the only way how we can decide is the theoretical position. Since our purpose was to test the possible indirect effects of perseverative thoughts on complaints, therefore, we chose NA → SHC direction. Nonetheless, the equivalence of the ‘original’ model (NA → SHC) and the alternative one (SHC → NA) suggests a

12

G. Kökönyei et al.

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

bidirectional relationship between emotional and physical distress. Further studies need to reveal when and how general distress and/or specific emotional states affect physical health, and vice versa, when and how general physical distress and/or specific complaints affect emotional life. Another limitation of the study was that known protective factors such as secure attachments to peer and family (Tremblay & Sullivan, 2010) or school-related sense of coherence (Modin, Ostberg, Toivanen, & Sundell, 2011) were not included. Effortful control as a temperament factor would have moderated the mediation between rumination and complaints, as it has been demonstrated for depressive symptoms in a followup study in a sample of 7th through 10th grades adolescents (Verstraeten, Vasey, Raes, & Bijttebier, 2009). Conclusions The health of adolescents has been demonstrated to be influenced by a range of social determinants, both structural (e.g. social inequality) and the dynamic features of the social environment (e.g. school environment) (Viner et al., 2012). Our results indicate that besides interpersonal factors (e.g. peer and family related functioning) and static factor (gender and age), interindividual differences of health in adolescence are determined by intrapersonal functioning as well. The main aims of this paper were to shed light on how perseverative thoughts could influence symptom reporting and experiencing. Cognitive and emotional intrapersonal mechanisms have been overlooked because of the attention given to addressing social determinants of health, but should have been studied in relation to health, because they may be modifiable much easier than disadvantaged social circumstances of the individual. Our results draw attention to intra-individual characteristics, namely cognitive and emotional processes in health. Previous research has mainly focused on rumination and worry as cognitive vulnerabilities in the development of depressive and anxiety symptoms and disorders and associated with attentional and interpretational biases concerning events, symptoms and distress (NolenHoeksema et al., 2008). However, our results highlight the possible role of perseverative thoughts in frequent somatic complaints in adolescence. Future works need to reveal how these processes are related to other aspects of health, and whether they are gender-dependent. Funding Gyöngyi Kökönyei and Zsolt Demetrovics acknowledge financial support of the János Bolyai Research Fellowship awarded by the Hungarian Academy of Sciences. This work was supported by the Hungarian Scientific Research Fund [grant number: 83884].

References Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004 Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Hogan, M. E., Whitehouse, W. G., Rose, D. T., Robinson, M. S., … Lapkin, J. B. (2000). The temple-wisconsin cognitive vulnerability to depression project:

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

Psychology & Health

13

Lifetime history of axis I psychopathology in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 403–418. Borkovec, T. D. (1994). The nature, functions, and origins of worry. In G. C. L. Davey & F. Tallis (Eds.), Worrying: Perspectives on theory, assessment and treatment. Wiley series in clinical psychology (pp. 5–33). Oxford: Wiley. Brans, K., Koval, P., Verduyn, P., Lim, Y. L., & Kuppens, P. (2013). The regulation of negative and positive affect in daily life. Emotion, 13, 926–939. doi: 2013-19076-001[pii]10.1037/ a0032400 Brosschot, J. F. (2002). Cognitive-emotional sensitization and somatic health complaints. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 113–121. doi:10.1111/1467-9450.00276 Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006). The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 113–124. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074 Brosschot, J. F., Pieper, S., & Thayer, J. F. (2005). Expanding stress theory: Prolonged activation and perseverative cognition. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 1043–1049. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.04.008 Brosschot, J. F., & van der Doef, M. (2006). Daily worrying and somatic health complaints: Testing the effectiveness of a simple worry reduction intervention. Psychology & Health, 21, 19–31. doi:10.1080/14768320500105346 Brosschot, J. F., Van Dijk, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2007). Daily worry is related to low heart rate variability during waking and the subsequent nocturnal sleep period. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63, 39–47. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.016 Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newsbury Park, CA: Sage. Calvete, E., Orue, I., & Hankin, B. L. (2013). Transactional relationships among cognitive vulnerabilities, stressors, and depressive symptoms in adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 399–410. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9691-y Charles, S. T., & Almeida, D. M. (2006). Daily reports of symptoms and negative affect: Not all symptoms are the same. Psychology & Health, 21(1), 1–17. doi:10.1080/14768320500129239 Chrousos, G. P. (2000). The stress response and immune function: Clinical implications – The 1999 Novera H. Spector Lecture. Neuroimmunomodulation: Perspectives at the New Millennium, 917, 38–67. Cohen, S., Alper, C. M., Doyle, W. J., Treanor, J. J., & Turner, R. B. (2006). Positive emotional style predicts resistance to illness after experimental exposure to rhinovirus or influenza a virus. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 809–815. doi:01.psy.0000245867.92364.3c[pii]10.1097/ 01.psy.0000245867.92364.3c Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Turner, R. B., Alper, C. M., & Skoner, D. P. (2003). Emotional style and susceptibility to the common cold. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 652–657. Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 386–396. Dantzer, R. (2005). Somatization: A psychoneuroimmune perspective. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 947–952. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.03.011 Dantzer, R., Bluthe, R. M., Gheusi, G., Cremona, S., Laye, S., Parnet, P., & Kelley, K. W. (1998). Molecular basis of sickness behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 856, 132–138. Dantzer, R., Bluthe, R. M., Laye, S., Bret-Dibat, J. L., Parnet, P., & Kelley, K. W. (1998). Cytokines and sickness behavior. Neuroimmunomodulation: Molecular Aspects, Integrative Systems, and Clinical Advances, 840, 586–590. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09597.x

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

14

G. Kökönyei et al.

De Gucht, V., & Heiser, W. (2003). Alexithymia and somatisation – A quantitative review of the literature. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54, 425–434. doi:10.1016/s0022-3999(02) 00467-1 Di Schiena, R., Luminet, O., & Philippot, P. (2011). Adaptive and maladaptive rumination in alexithymia and their relation with depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 10–14. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.037 Ehring, T., & Watkins, E. R. (2008). Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic process. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 1, 192–205. doi:10.1680/ijct.2008.1.3.192 Eriksen, H. R., & Ursin, H. (2004). Subjective health complaints, sensitization, and sustained cognitive activation (stress). Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 56, 445–448. doi:10.1016/ s0022-3999(03)00629-9 Finch, J. F., Baranik, L. E., Liu, Y., & West, S. G. (2012). Physical health, positive and negative affect, and personality: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 537–545. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.05.013 Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1045–1062. doi:10.1037/a0013262 Friedlander, L., Lumley, M. A., Farchione, T., & Doyal, G. (1997). Testing the alexithymia hypothesis: Physiological and subjective responses during relaxation and stress. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185, 233–239. doi:10.1097/00005053-199704000-00003 Garnefski, N., Koopman, H., Kraaij, V., & ten Cate, R. (2009). Brief report: Cognitive emotion regulation strategies and psychological adjustment in adolescents with a chronic disease. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 449–454. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.01.003 Garnefski, N., Rieffe, C., Jellesma, F., Terwogt, M. M., & Kraaij, V. (2007). Cognitive emotion regulation strategies and emotional problems in 9–11-year-old children – The development of an instrument. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 16(1), 1–9. doi:10.1007/s00787006-0562-3 Genet, J. J., & Siemer, M. (2012). Rumination moderates the effects of daily events on negative mood: Results from a diary study. Emotion, 12, 1329–1339. doi:10.1037/a0028070 Ghandour, R. M., Overpeck, M. D., Huang, Z. H. J., Kogan, M. D., & Scheidt, P. C. (2004). Headache, stomachache, backache, and morning fatigue among adolescent girls in the United States – Associations with behavioral, sociodemographic, and. environmental factors. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 158, 797–803. doi:10.1001/archpedi.158.8.797 Gianferante, D., Thoma, M. V., Hanlin, L., Chen, X., Breines, J. G., Zoccola, P. M., & Rohleder, N. (2014). Post-stress rumination predicts HPA axis responses to repeated acute stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 49, 244–252. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.021 Gobina, I., Valimaa, R., Tynjala, J., Villberg, J., Villerusa, A., Iannotti, R. J., … Grp, H. M. U. W. (2011). The medicine use and corresponding subjective health complaints among adolescents, a cross-national survey. Pharmacoepidemiology and Drug Safety, 20, 424–431. doi:10.1002/ pds.2102 Gyollai, A., Simor, P., Koteles, F., & Demetrovics, Z. (2011). Psychometric properties of the Hungarian version of the original and the short form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Neuropsychopharmacol Hung, 13, 73–79. Hagquist, C. (2009). Psychosomatic health problems among adolescents in Swedenare the time trends gender related. European Journal of Public Health, 19, 331–336. doi:10.1093/eurpub/ ckp031 Hagquist, C., & Andrich, D. (2004). Measuring subjective health among adolescents in Sweden – A Rasch-analysis of the HBSC – Instrument. Social Indicators Research, 68, 201–220. doi:10.1023/B:SOCI.0000025593.97559.7f

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

Psychology & Health

15

Hong, R. Y. (2007). Worry and rumination: Differential associations with anxious and depressive symptoms and coping behavior. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 277–290. doi:10.1016/ j.brat.2006.03.006 Huffziger, S., Ebner-Priemer, U., Zamoscik, V., Reinhard, I., Kirsch, P., & Kuehner, C. (2013). Effects of mood and rumination on cortisol levels in daily life: An ambulatory assessment study in remitted depressed patients and healthy controls. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 2258–2267. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.04.014 Hurrelmann, K., Engel, U., Holler, B., & Nordlohne, E. (1988). Failure in school, family conflicts, and psychosomatic disorders in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 11, 237–249. doi:10.1016/s0140-1971(88)80007-1 Jellesma, F., Meerum Terwogt, M., Reijntjes, A., Rieffe, C., & Stegge, H. (2005). De vragenlijst Non-Productieve Denkprocessen voor Kinderen (NPDK): Piekeren en Rumineren [Non-productive thought questionnaire for kids: Worry and rumination]. Kind en Adolescent, 26, 368–378. Jellesma, F., Rieffe, C., & Terwogt, M. M. (2006). Do nonproductive thoughts contribute to more somatic complaints in children? Psychology & Health, 21, 72–73 Jellesma, F. C., Rieffe, C., & Terwogt, M. M. (2007). The somatic complaint list: Validation of a self-report questionnaire assessing somatic complaints in children. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 63, 399–401. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.01.017 Jellesma, F. C., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Kneepkens, C. M. F. (2006). Somatic complaints and health care use in children: Mood, emotion awareness and sense of coherence. Social Science and Medicine, 63, 2640–2648. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.07.004 Jellesma, F. C., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Westenberg, P. M. (2008). Do parents reinforce somatic complaints in their children? Health Psychology, 27, 280–285. doi:10.1037/02786133.27.1280 Jellesma, F. C., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Westenberg, P. M. (2011). Children’s sense of coherence and trait emotional intelligence: A longitudinal study exploring the development of somatic complaints. Psychology & Health, 26, 307–320. doi:10.1080/08870440903411021 Jellesma, F. C., Verkuil, B., & Brosschot, J. F. (2009). Postponing worrisome thoughts in children: The effects of a postponement intervention on perseverative thoughts, emotions and somatic complaints. Social Science and Medicine, 69, 278–284. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.04.031 Johnson, D. P., & Whisman, M. A. (2013). Gender differences in rumination: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 367–374. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.019 Johnson, J. A., Lavoie, K. L., Bacon, S. L., Carlson, L. E., & Campbell, T. S. (2012). The effect of trait rumination on adaptation to repeated stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 74, 258–262. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e31824c3ef2 Kok, B. E., Coffey, K. A., Cohn, M. A., Catalino, L. I., Vacharkulksemsuk, T., Algoe, S. B., … Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). How positive emotions build physical health: Perceived positive social connections account for the upward spiral between positive emotions and vagal tone. Psychological Science, 24, 1123–1132. doi:10.1177/0956797612470827 Kraaij, V., & Garnefski, N. (2012). Coping and depressive symptoms in adolescents with a chronic medical condition: A search for intervention targets. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 1593–1600. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.06.007 Kroenke, K., & Spitzer, R. L. (1998). Gender differences in the reporting of physical and somatoform symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 150–155. Kross, E., Ayduk, O., & Mischel, W. (2005). When asking “why” does not hurt. Distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative emotions. Psychological Science, 16, 709–715. doi:PSCI1600[pii]10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01600.x

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

16

G. Kökönyei et al.

Kross, E., Duckworth, A., Ayduk, O., Tsukayama, E., & Mischel, W. (2011). The effect of self-distancing on adaptive versus maladaptive self-reflection in children. Emotion, 11, 1032–1039. doi:10.1037/a0021787 Kross, E., Gard, D., Deldin, P., Clifton, J., & Ayduk, O. (2012). “Asking why” from a distance: Its cognitive and emotional consequences for people with major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 559–569. doi:10.1037/a0028808 Költő, A., & Kökönyei, G. (2011). Szubjektív jóllét [Subjective well-being]. In Á. Németh & A. Költő (Eds.), Serdülőkorúfiatalok egészsége és életmódja 2010 [Health and health behaviour in school-aged children 2010] (pp. 67–76). Budapest: Országos Gyermekegészségügyi Intézet. Legerstee, J. S., Garnefski, N., Verhulst, F. C., & Utens, E. M. W. J. (2011). Cognitive coping in anxiety-disordered adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 34, 319–326. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.04.008 Lohaus, A., Vierhaus, M., Frevert, A., Kreienborg, A. K., Laker, E. F., Pelster, S., & Schilling, C. (2013). Rumination and symptom reports in children and adolescents: Results of a crosssectional and experimental study. Psychol Health, 28, 1032–1045. doi:10.1080/ 08870446.2013.780606 Lyubomirsky, S., Tucker, K. L., Caldwell, N. D., & Berg, K. (1999). Why ruminators are poor problem solvers: Clues from the phenomenology of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1041–1060. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1041 Maier, S. F., & Watkins, L. R. (1998). Cytokines for psychologists: Implications of bidirectional immune-to-brain communication for understanding behavior, mood, and cognition. Psychological Review, 105, 83–107. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.105.1.83 Martin, L. L., Tesser, A., & McIntosh, W. D. (1993). Wanting but not having: The effects of unattained goals on thoughts and feelings. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control. Century psychology series (pp. 552–572): Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. McCullough, M. E., Orsulak, P., Brandon, A., & Akers, L. (2007). Rumination, fear, and cortisol: An in vivo study of interpersonal transgressions. Health Psychol, 26, 126–132. doi:200623340-016[pii]10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.126 McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease – Allostasis and allostatic load. Neuroimmunomodulation: Molecular Aspects. Integrative Systems, and Clinical Advances, 840, 33–44. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x McLaughlin, K. A., Borkovec, T. D., & Sibrava, N. J. (2007). The effects of worry and rumination on affect states and cognitive activity. Behavior Therapy, 38, 23–38. doi:S0005-7894(06) 00066-9[pii]10.1016/j.beth.2006.03.003 McLean, C. P., Asnaani, A., Litz, B. T., & Hofmann, S. G. (2011). Gender differences in anxiety disorders: Prevalence, course of illness, comorbidity and burden of illness. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 1027–1035. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.03.006 Michl, L. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Shepherd, K., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2013). Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: Longitudinal evidence in early adolescents and adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 339–352. doi:10.1037/a0031994 Miers, A. C., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., Cowan, R., & Linden, W. (2007). The relation between anger coping strategies, anger mood and somatic complaints in children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 653–664. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9120-9 Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2008a). Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: An experience sampling study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 314–323. doi:10.1037/ 0021-843x.117.2.314 Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2008b). Ruminative self-focus, negative life events, and negative affect. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 1034–1039. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.06.004

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

Psychology & Health

17

Modin, B., Ostberg, V., Toivanen, S., & Sundell, K. (2011). Psychosocial working conditions, school sense of coherence and subjective health complaints. A multilevel analysis of ninth grade pupils in the Stockholm area. Journal of Adolescence, 34, 129–139. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.01.004 Moreno, C., Sanchez-Queija, I., Munoz-Tinoco, V., de Matos, M. G., Dallago, L., Ter Bogt, T., … Grp, H. P. C. F. (2009). Cross-national associations between parent and peer communication and psychological complaints. International Journal of Public Health, 54, 235–242. doi:10.1007/s00038-009-5415-7 Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2007). Mplus user guide. 5th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. Nath, P., & Pradhan, R. K. (2012). Influence of positive affect on physical health and psychological well-being: Examining the mediating role of psychological resilience. Journal of Health Management, 14, 161–174. doi:10.1177/097206341201400206 Natvig, G. K., Albrektsen, G., Anderssen, N., & Qvarnstrom, U. (1999). School-related stress and psychosomatic symptoms among school adolescents. Journal of School Health, 69, 362–368. Nemiah, J. C. (1996). Alexithymia: Present, past- and future? Psychosomatic Medicine, 58, 217–218. Nixon, C. L., Linkie, C. A., Coleman, P. K., & Fitch, C. (2011). Peer relational victimization and somatic complaints during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 49, 294–299. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.12.018 Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400–424. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x O’Connor, D. B., Walker, S., Hendrickx, H., Talbot, D., & Schaefer, A. (2013). Stress-related thinking predicts the cortisol awakening response and somatic symptoms in healthy adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 438–446. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.07.004 Pettit, J. W., Kline, J. P., Gencoz, T., Gencoz, F., & Joiner, T. E. (2001). Are happy people healthier? the specific role of positive affect in predicting self-reported health symptoms. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 521–536. doi:10.1006/jrpe.2001.2327 Pieper, S., Brosschot, J. F., van der Leeden, R., & Thayer, J. F. (2010). Prolonged cardiac effects of momentary assessed stressful events and worry episodes. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72, 570–577. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181dbc0e9 Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 925–971. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925 Radstaak, M., Geurts, S. A. E., Brosschot, J. F., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Kompier, M. A. J. (2011). The role of affect and rumination in cardiovascular recovery from stress. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 81, 237–244. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.06.017 Ravens-Sieberer, U., Torsheim, T., Hetland, J., Vollebergh, W., Cavallo, F., Jericek, H., … Grp, H. P. H. F. (2009). Subjective health, symptom load and quality of life of children and adolescents in Europe. International Journal of Public Health, 54, 151–159. doi:10.1007/s00038009-5406-8 Saps, M., Seshadri, R., Stainberg, M., Schaffer, G., Marshall, B. M., & Di Lorenzo, C. (2009). A prospective school-based study of abdominal pain and other common somatic complaints in children. Journal of Pediatrics, 154, 322–326. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.09.047 Sentenac, M., Arnaud, C., Gavin, A., Molcho, M., Gabhainn, S. N., & Godeau, E. (2012). Peer victimization among school-aged children with chronic conditions. Epidemiologic Reviews, 34, 120–128. doi:10.1093/epirev/mxr024 Sifneos, P. E. (1973). The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 22, 255–262.

Downloaded by [Massachusetts PRIM Board] at 14:43 08 April 2015

18

G. Kökönyei et al.

Stauder, A., & Konkoly Thege, B. (2006). Az Észlelt Stressz Kérdőív (PSS) magyar verziójának jellemzői [Characteristics of the Hungarian version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS)]. Mentálhigiéné és Pszichoszomatika, 7, 203–216. Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Parker, J. D. (1991). The alexithymia construct: A potential paradigm for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatics, 32, 153–164. Tremblay, I., & Sullivan, M. J. L. (2010). Attachment and pain outcomes in adolescents: The mediating role of pain catastrophizing and anxiety. Journal of Pain, 11, 160–171. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2009.06.015 Tzavara, C., Tzonou, A., Zervas, I., Ravens-Sieberer, U., Dimitrakaki, C., & Tountas, Y. (2012). Reliability and validity of the KIDSCREEN-52 health-related quality of life questionnaire in a Greek adolescent population. Ann Gen Psychiatry, 11, 3. doi:1744-859X-11-3[pii]10.1186/ 1744-859X-11-3 Ursin, H., & Eriksen, H. R. (2004). The cognitive activation theory of stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 567–592. doi:10.1016/s0306-4530(03)00091-x Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., de Beurs, D. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2009). Effects of explicit and implicit perseverative cognition on cardiac recovery after cognitive stress. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74, 220–228. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.09.003 Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., Gebhardt, W. A., & Thayer, J. F. (2010). When worries make you sick: A review of perseverative cognition, the default stress response and somatic health. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 1, 87–118. Verstraeten, K., Vasey, M. W., Raes, F., & Bijttebier, P. (2009). Temperament and risk for depressive symptoms in adolescence: Mediation by rumination and moderation by effortful control. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 349–361. doi:10.1007/s10802-008-9293-x Viner, R. M., Ozer, E. M., Denny, S., Marmot, M., Resnick, M., Fatusi, A., & Currie, C. (2012). Adolescent health 2 adolescence and the social determinants of health. Lancet, 379, 1641–1652. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(12)60149-4 Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychological Review, 96, 234–254. doi:10.1037//0033295x.96.2.234 Wehmer, F., Brejnak, C., Lumley, M., & Stettner, L. (1995). Alexithymia and physiological reactivity to emotion-provoking visual scenes. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 351–357. doi:10.1097/00005053-199506000-00001 Weissman, M. M., Bland, R., Joyce, P. R., Newman, S., Wells, J. E., & Wittchen, H. U. (1993). Sex differences in rates of depression: Cross-national perspectives. Journal of Affective Disorders, 29, 77–84. doi:10.1016/0165-0327(93)90025-f Zautra, A., Smith, B., Affleck, G., & Tennen, H. (2001). Examinations of chronic pain and affect relationships: Applications of a dynamic model of affect. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 786–795. doi:10.1037//0022-006x.69.5.786 Zoccola, P. M., & Dickerson, S. S. (2012). Assessing the relationship between rumination and cortisol: A review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 73(1), 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.03.007 Zoccola, P. M., Figueroa, W. S., Rabideau, E. M., Woody, A., & Benencia, F. (2014). Differential effects of poststressor rumination and distraction on cortisol and C-reactive protein. Health Psychology, 33, 1606–1609. doi:2014-02487-001[pii]10.1037/hea0000019

Perseverative thoughts and subjective health complaints in adolescence: Mediating effects of perceived stress and negative affects.

Stable tendency to perseverative thoughts such as trait rumination and worry can influence somatic health. The purpose of the study was to investigate...
241KB Sizes 0 Downloads 13 Views