Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 2016, Vol. 7, No. 1, 15–27

© 2015 American Psychological Association 1949-2715/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/per0000140

Personality Disorder Models and Their Coverage of Interpersonal Problems Trevor F. Williams and Leonard J. Simms

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

University at Buffalo, The State University of New York Interpersonal dysfunction is a defining feature of personality disorders (PDs) and can serve as a criterion for comparing PD models. In this study, the interpersonal coverage of 4 competing PD models was examined using a sample of 628 current or recent psychiatric patients who completed the NEO Personality Inventory-3 First Half (NEO-PI-3FH; McCrae & Costa, 2007), Personality Inventory for the DSM–5 (PID-5; Krueger et al., 2012), Computerized Adaptive Test of Personality Disorder-Static Form (CAT-PD-SF; Simms et al., 2011), and Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Personality Questionnaire (SCID-II PQ; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1995). Participants also completed the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems-Short Circumplex (IIP-SC; Soldz, Budman, Demby, & Merry, 1995) to assess interpersonal dysfunction. Analyses compared the severity and style of interpersonal problems that characterize PD models. Previous research with DSM–5 Section II and III models was generally replicated. Extraversion and Agreeableness facets related to the most well defined interpersonal problems across normal-range and pathological traits. Pathological trait models provided more coverage of dominance problems, whereas normal-range traits covered nonassertiveness better. These results suggest that more work may be needed to reconcile descriptions of personality pathology at the level of specific constructs. Keywords: DSM–5, personality disorder, interpersonal, five-factor model, facets

Personality disorders (PDs) frequently are described in terms of interpersonal dysfunction (Hopwood, Wright, Ansell, & Pincus, 2013; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013) that manifests quite variably (e.g., Pincus & Wiggins, 1990; Monsen, Hagtvet, Havik, & Eilertsen, 2006). For instance, Schizoid PD is defined by “detachment from social relationships,” (p. 653) whereas Histrionic PD involves “excessive emotionality and attention seeking” (APA, 2013, p. 667). Thus, in evaluating and comparing competing PD classification models, an important consideration is how each accounts for such diverse expressions of interpersonal dysfunction. Considerable research has examined Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) PDs in this regard (e.g., Soldz, Budman, Demby, & Merry, 1993); however, less is known about contemporary PD models (e.g., Widiger, 2011; Simms et al., 2011). The present study fills this gap in the literature by comparing the interpersonal impairments associated with several prominent PD models.

Models of Personality Disorder PD models can be broadly differentiated by how they organize phenotypic variability in PD presentation. The official DSM–5 PD classification chapter (i.e., Section II; APA, 2013) organizes this variability via 10 categories supported primarily by clinical theory and expertise (e.g., Kendler, 2009). Despite this system’s endurance, it has been widely criticized as being inconsistent with research on the structure of personality pathology (e.g., Widiger & Samuel, 2005; Skodol, 2012). Moreover, these traditional categorical PDs have limited validity and usefulness because of their excessive co-occurrence, within-diagnosis heterogeneity, arbitrary cut points, and inability to describe all PD patients (e.g., Clark, 2007; Skodol, 2012). A growing number of researchers advocate reorganizing the phenotypic variability in PD with empirically derived trait models (e.g., Krueger, Deringer, Markon, Watson, & Skodol, 2012), which consist of distinct and homogenous dimensions (Clark, 2007). Despite attempts to implement such a system (e.g., Krueger et al., 2012), disagreement remains concerning issues such as trait uni- versus bipolarity (Samuel, 2011; Krueger et al., 2011), the nature and number of lower-order PD traits (Simms et al., 2011), and whether these traits should focus on normal- or abnormalrange personality features (Widiger, 2011). In part because of these disagreements, the DSM–5 Personality and PD Work Group’s proposed trait model was placed in Section III of DSM–5 (“Emerging Measures and Models”; APA, 2013) to spur further research. As described in the first paragraph, interpersonal dysfunction is a central feature of PD; the convergence and divergence of PD models in their relations to such dysfunction can thus inform ongoing debates. A comprehensive interpersonal dysfunction model capable of making subtle differentiations in how such dysfunction manifests would facilitate this process.

This article was published Online First July 13, 2015. Trevor F. Williams and Leonard J. Simms, Department of Psychology, University at Buffalo, The State University of New York. This study was supported by a research grant to L. J. Simms from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH080086). We thank Kristin Naragon-Gainey for feedback on a draft of this article, as well as Lew Goldberg, David Watson, John Roberts, John Welte, William Calabrese, Jane Rotterman, Monica Rudick, Aidan Wright, Wern How Yam, and Kerry Zelazny for their support of the broader project from which these data were drawn. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Trevor F. Williams, Department of Psychology, Park Hall 226, University at Buffalo, The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY 14260. E-mail: [email protected] 15

16

WILLIAMS AND SIMMS

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Conceptualizing Interpersonal Dysfunction One model with an internal structure and content domain appropriate for comparing PD models is the Interpersonal Circle (IPC; Leary, 1957). The IPC is a circular model defined by two orthogonal axes, which can be broadly described as agency and communion. Agency is a dimension with themes of power, mastery, and assertion on one end versus weakness, failure, and submissiveness on the other (e.g., dominance vs. submissiveness). Communion contrasts themes of intimacy, union, and solidarity with remoteness, hostility, and disaffiliation (e.g., warmth vs. coldness; Pincus, Lukowitsky, & Wright, 2010). All points within the IPC reflect blends of these two dimensions (e.g., colddominance), such that external indicators (e.g., PD measures) related to agency and communion can be described precisely by their location in this space. Relations to interpersonal dysfunction may be described by their specificity (i.e., how clearly a construct relates to one region of IPC), style (i.e., which region it relates to), and severity (i.e., degree of relation to nonspecific interpersonal distress; Gurtman, 1992). Describing components of a PD model with such nuance allows the IPC to provide a useful basis for comparing the amount and type of interpersonal dysfunction associated with different models for classifying and assessing personality pathology.

Interpersonal Problems and Personality Pathology Personality pathology relates to general interpersonal distress (i.e., severity; Wright et al., 2012); however, both PDs and traits vary in the degree to which they can be characterized by a specific class of interpersonal problems (i.e., style; Soldz et al., 1993). Some expressions of personality pathology cannot be described well by one style of interpersonal problems. For instance, borderline individuals exhibit quite specific interpersonal problems at times, but as a group are heterogeneous and experience great variability in problems across time (Wright, Hallquist, Beeney, & Pilkonis, 2013; Wright, Hallquist, Morse, et al., 2013). In contrast, other forms of personality pathology relate consistently to very specific classes of interpersonal problems. Histrionic PD, for example, is related to warm-dominant interpersonal problems (i.e., being intrusive) and Avoidant PD relates to cold-submissive problems (i.e., social avoidance; Soldz et al., 1993). Consistent with this research, the traits of withdrawal and anhedonia relate to cold-submissive problems, whereas attention-seeking relates to warm-dominant problems (Hopwood, Koonce, & Morey, 2009; Wright et al., 2012). Research has identified PD constructs in all quadrants of the IPC, but the degree to which specific PD models are represented in each quadrant appears to vary considerably. Current PD models have few components associated with warm interpersonal problems. Only two PDs (Histrionic and Dependent) consistently relate to warm problems (Pincus & Wiggins, 1990; Soldz et al., 1993; Monsen et al., 2006) and pathological trait systems do not provide additional depth in coverage (Hopwood et al., 2009; Simonsen & Simonsen, 2009; Wright et al., 2012). In contrast, normal-range personality traits may cover warm problems at the expense of cold ones (e.g., Soldz et al., 1993), although these warm problems may be less severe (Hopwood et al., 2013). In particular, Five-Factor Model Agreeableness may relate to problems with dependency that are not covered by pathological trait models (e.g., Gore & Pincus, 2013).

Notably, the PD-IIP literature exhibits limitations that may obscure links between personality pathology and interpersonal problems. For instance, studies vary in whether the specificity and severity of a PD construct’s interpersonal dysfunction are considered. In addition, faceted personality trait measures have not been examined in patient samples with the IIP, leaving open questions regarding range-restriction and generalizability. Furthermore, normal-range lower-order traits (i.e., facets) relevant to recent PD model proposals (e.g., Widiger, 2011) have not been examined with the IIP in any sample. Thus, an important step forward would be to examine how currently prominent PD models relate to the IIP in a patient sample.

The Present Study The DSM–5 Section II PDs (APA, 2013) and three currently prominent PD trait models (Krueger et al., 2012; Simms et al., 2011; Widiger, 2011) were compared through the relations of their most commonly used measures to the IIP in a psychiatric patient sample. The aim of this research was to examine the similarities and differences in how these PD measures relate to the IIP, particularly at the level of disorders and lower-order traits. This study marks the first such comparison within a single sample and the first exploration of facet-IIP relations for two measures (Simms et al., 2011; Widiger, 2011). In addition, the measures used in this study allowed differences between normal-range and pathological traits to be more fully understood. Finally, this study used data from a patient sample, which allowed past findings to be reexamined in a more ecologically valid context. PD measures were characterized by the severity and specificity of their components’ relations to interpersonal problems, as well as by the predominate style of problems to which their components related. Following from this, several predictions were made. Our first hypothesis (H1) was that past findings relating traditional categorical PDs (i.e., DSM–5 Section II) and the emerging trait model (i.e., Section III) to interpersonal problems would be replicated. Second (H2), as Extraversion and Agreeableness are considered alternative interpretations of agency and communion (e.g., McCrae & Costa, 1989), we predicted that facets from these domains would relate to the IIP with the most specificity. Finally, at the facet-level, some differences among all trait measures were expected; however, major differences were hypothesized (H3) to revolve around range of measurement. Pathological traits were predicted to relate more strongly to problem severity (H3a) and cold interpersonal problems than were normal-range traits (H3b), which were expected to cover warm interpersonal problems most effectively of all models (H3c).

Method Participants and Procedures As part of a larger study (Simms et al., 2011), 695 current or recent psychiatric patients were recruited using flyers distributed across Western New York mental health clinics. Each participant attended a 4-hr session, receiving $50 plus transportation reimbursement. Participants completed structured interviews and computerized self-report measures. After removing data influenced by inconsistent responding, nonrandom missingness, or questionable

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

PERSONALITY DISORDER AND INTERPERSONAL DYSFUNCTION

in-session behaviors (e.g., acute substance use), 628 participants remained. Because of questionnaire battery length and time constraints, some participants did not complete all measures, limiting the sample size for each analysis. In the present study, participants who did not complete the IIP-SC and at least one personality measure were excluded from analyses. The remaining participants (N ⫽ 438) averaged 42 years of age, were 67% female, and identified primarily as Caucasian (69%) or African American (29%). In addition, 2% of the sample identified as American Indian or Alaskan Native and ⬍1% as Asian or Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander. The majority of participants were either currently in treatment (79.68%) or had been in the past 1 to 2 years (14%). Based on structured clinical interviews, 32% of participants met criteria for Major Depressive Disorder, 27% for Bipolar I, 2% for Bipolar II, 11% for Dysthymia, 10% for Agoraphobia, 17% for Social Anxiety Disorder, 23% for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder, 20% for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder, 44% for Generalized Anxiety Disorder, 22% for Alcohol Use Disorder, 18% for Substance Use Disorder, and 3% for Schizophrenia. Relative to the 190 participants who were excluded, included participants were more likely to be female (67% vs. 56%; ␹2(1, N ⫽ 627) ⫽ 7.25, p ⫽ .007), on average were 4.7 years younger (i.e., t(626) ⫽ 4.33, p ⬍ .001), and differed significantly in terms of race, ␹2(4, N ⫽ 624) ⫽ 24.71, p ⬍ .001. Specifically, African American participants made up a smaller proportion of the included group (29%) relative to excluded group (47%; ␹2(1, N ⫽ 624) ⫽ 19.16, p ⬍ .001). The included participants also reported significantly less personality pathology, t(599) ⫽ 3.09, p ⫽ .002, as indicated by the sum of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Personality Questionnaire (SCID-II PQ); however, the difference between participants was small (i.e., d ⫽ .27) and thus unlikely to affect the generalizability of our findings.

Measures Inventory of Interpersonal Problems-Short Circumplex (IIP-SC). The IIP-SC (Soldz et al., 1995) is a 32-item measure of interpersonal behaviors that individuals perform excessively (e.g., “I argue with other people too much”) or find difficult to do (e.g., “It is hard for me to feel close to other people”). Items form 8 four-item scales (Domineering, Vindictive, Cold, Socially Avoidant, Nonassertive, Exploitable, Overly Nurturant, and Intrusive) that correspond to equally spaced octants of the IPC. Use in previous patient samples (Soldz et al., 1995) indicates adequate internal consistency and adherence to circumplex structure (Alden, Wiggins, & Pincus, 1990; Gurtman, 1992; Monsen et al., 2006). As described above, all participants (438; 100%) in this subsample completed the IIP-SC. The median alpha coefficient was .78 (Range ⫽ .73–.90). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Personality Questionnaire (SCID-II PQ). The SCID-II PQ is a self-report screening questionnaire used for the SCID-II interview (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1995) for PDs that consists of 121 dichotomous (i.e., yes/no) items that are related to DSM PD criteria (e.g., APA, 2013). One limitation of this measure is that self-reported Antisocial PD (ASPD) symptoms cannot be scored, although conduct disorder can be. Past work supports the SCID-II PQ as a screening measure (e.g., Ullrich et al., 2008) for PDs and

17

as an indicator of personality pathology on its own (e.g., Ryder, Costa, & Bagby, 2007). In the present sample 337 (77%) participants completed the SCID-II PQ. Internal consistency varied by disorder (Mdn ⫽ .72, Range ⫽ .51–.86). Personality Inventory for DSM–5 (PID-5). The PID-5 (Krueger et al., 2012) is a 220-item questionnaire with 25 lowerorder scales, one for each of the DSM–5 Section III pathological personality traits. These lower-order facet scales can be organized into five domains (Negative Affectivity, Antagonism, Detachment, Disinhibition, and Psychoticism) that have been described as conceptually similar to the Personality Psychopathology-5 model (PSY-5; Watson, Stasik, Ro, & Clark, 2013). Each facet scale is measured by 4 to 14 items measured on a 4-point scale of 0 (very false or often false) to 3 (very true or often true). In the present sample, 435 (99%) participants completed the PID-5. The median facet alpha coefficient was .87 (Range ⫽ .75–.96). NEO Personality Inventory-3 First Half (NEO PI-3FH). The NEO-PI-3 (McCrae et al., 2005) is a revision of the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992) intended to improve readability and item-total correlations. Research examining problematic items yielded 37 replacement items, improving the measure’s psychometric properties without altering its structure (McCrae et al., 2005). The full measure consists of 240 statements that are rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). These items form 30 facet scales, with six facets for each of the higher-order domains (Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness). The NEO-PI-3FH consists of the first 120 items of the NEO-PI-R, with four items for each of the 30 facets. Despite being half the length of the original measure, relations to full-length scales are strong (Mdn ⫽ .91) and the overall-structure of the measure is preserved (McCrae & Costa, 2007). A total of 381 (87%) participants completed the NEO-PI3FH. For facets, the median alpha was .63 (Range ⫽ .40 –.80). Computerized Adaptive Test of Personality Disorder-Static Form (CAT-PD-SF). The CAT-PD-SF is a brief measure drawn from the full CAT-PD item pool. The CAT-PD item pool was generated as part of project to develop a comprehensive model and efficient measure of PD traits, guided by a PSY-5 framework (Simms et al., 2011). The project has resulted in a pool of 1,366 items that form scales for 33 lower-order PD traits. Because of the number of items, a balanced incomplete block design (BIBD) incorporating “planned-missingness” was used to reduce the number of items a single participant must complete (for details see Simms et al., 2011; Wright & Simms, 2014). This procedure resulted in a median sample size of 195 (range ⫽ 183–218; 42–50% of sample) for individual traits for the present study. These participants all rated a subset of 216 items that have been used to define a static form (CAT-PD-SF) of each of the 33 CAT-PD traits. A 5-point scale ranging from 1 (very untrue of me) to 5 (very true of me) is used. In the current study the median coefficient alpha was .85 (Range ⫽ .80 –.89). Preliminary research suggests that the CAT-PD-SF scales relate strongly to the intended a priori PSY-5 structure (Wright & Simms, 2014).

Data Analyses All trait and PD scales were first correlated with the octant scales of the IIP-SC; however, the IIP-SC’s structure allows more detailed analyses. IIP-SC octant scale interrelations are dictated by

WILLIAMS AND SIMMS

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

18

the measure’s specific geometric structure (Gurtman, 1992), such that adjacent scales (e.g., Vindictive and Cold) are more closely related than scales located further apart (e.g., Intrusive and Nonassertive). The specificity of these interrelations allows the discriminant validity of constructs (e.g., PD traits) to be examined via their patterns of correlations across IIP-SC scales. The Structural Summary Method (SSM) for circumplex data (e.g., Wright, Pincus, Conroy, & Hilsenroth, 2009) quantifies these patterns. The SSM models correlations between the IIP-SC and external constructs as a cosine curve (see Figure 1) that is described by elevation, amplitude, R2, and angular displacement parameters. Elevation is the average correlation across octants for a scale and is an indicator of general interpersonal distress (i.e., severity). Amplitude compliments elevation; it is the difference between the midpoint (i.e., mean correlation) and peak of the curve, with large values indicating that a scale relates to specific problematic behaviors. The degree to which a scale’s observed pattern of correlations matches a prototypical cosine curve is quantified by the R2 statistic. Given that a trait has a prototypical profile of correlations and shows specific relations with the IIPSC, angular displacement will indicate the area of the IPC that is most descriptive of the trait (i.e., style of interpersonal problems). Recent developments in the use of the SSM (Wright et al., 2012; Zimmerman & Wright, 2015) provide standards for evaluating parameters and inference testing. Wright et al. (2012; Zimmerman & Wright, 2015) conducted a review of studies using the SSM and concluded that elevation and amplitude values ⬎ |.10| should be considered small effects, values ⬎ |.20| be regarded as moderate, and values ⬎ |.30| be interpreted as large. In addition, they recommend only interpreting amplitude and angular displacement when R2 is above .70. Beyond this, Zimmerman & Wright (2015) present a bootstrapping method (and R package) for placing confidence intervals (CIs) around SSM parameters, permitting hypothesis testing and additional precision. Monte Carlo simulations

Figure 1.

indicated that CIs for amplitude and angular displacement are influenced by sample size, as well as parameter magnitude. Because of this, Zimmerman and Wright recommend reporting the probability of obtaining an accurate CI (from R package) and if low (i.e., ⬍.50), that CIs not be considered interpretable. Elevation, amplitude, R2, angular displacement, and associated CIs (for all parameters except R2) were computed for all traits and DSM PDs. Models were compared in terms of the number and proportion of components above cutoffs for elevation and amplitude, reflecting the severity and specificity of interpersonal problems they relate to. Finally, the angular displacement values across models were compared to examine hypotheses regarding differences in the interpersonal coverage of PD models.

Results Bivariate Correlations To aid interpretation, Table 1 contains domain-level trait intercorrelations (facet-level intercorrelations are available upon request). Table 2 presents descriptive statistics and coefficient alphas for PD construct scales, as well as bivariate correlations with all octant scales of the IIP-SC. Average IIP-SC octant correlations for the PID-5 (range ⫽ .23 to .42), CAT-PD-SF (range ⫽ .18 to .37), and SCID-II PQ (range ⫽ .14 to 35) peaked at moderate strength, whereas the NEO-PI-3FH was more weakly related to the IIP-SC (range ⫽ ⫺.13 to .02). Scale-level correlations were examined to understand variability in PD model relations to IIP-SC octants. Most CAT-PD-SF, PID-5, and SCID-II PQ scales correlated positively and significantly with at least one IIP-SC scale; however, many NEO-PI-3FH scales were best characterized by significant negative correlations with the IIP-SC. In this study, NEO-PI-3FH scales initially were scored in the direction suggested by their scale names (e.g., a high score on

Circumplex structural summary method parameters.

PERSONALITY DISORDER AND INTERPERSONAL DYSFUNCTION

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Table 1 Domain-Level Intercorrelations for Personality Traits Measures Measure

NA

DET

ANT

DIS

PSY

Neuroticism Extraversion Agreeableness Conscientiousness Openness

.78 ⫺.35 ⫺.27 ⫺.46 ⫺.05

.50 ⴚ.73 ⫺.19 ⫺.39 ⫺.31

.12 .09 ⴚ.67 ⫺.19 ⫺.07

.60 ⫺.29 ⫺.34 ⴚ.73 ⫺.04

.40 ⫺.20 ⫺.34 ⫺.40 .11

Note. Pairwise N ⫽ 379. NEO-PI-3FH domain scale names are listed in the first column and abbreviated PID-5 domain scale names are listed above columns. NA ⫽ Negative affect; DET ⫽ Detachment; ANT ⫽ Antagonism; DIS ⫽ Disinhibition; PSY ⫽ Psychoticism. Correlations ⬎ .10 are significant at p ⬍ .05. Correlations ⱖ |.50| are in bold.

“Altruism” indicates trait altruism). However, NEO traits are considered bipolar, implying low scores are meaningful as well (e.g., Samuel, 2011). To focus on dysfunction, all NEO-PI-3FH scales with negative average correlations with the IIP-SC were reversescored. These re-keyed traits were used in the remaining analyses and marked with minus signs. Reverse scoring these scales altered the average correlations and standard deviations in Table 2, therefore adjusted means (range ⫽ .12 to .27) and standard deviations are reported below the original values. Although the mean values still were lower than those for the other measures, they were considerably higher than the original NEO-PI-3FH values.

SSM Parameters SSM parameters and associated CIs are summarized in Table 3. Median elevation parameters for CAT-PD-SF facets (Mdn ⫽ .29) and PID-5 facets (Mdn ⫽ .34) indicate relations to severe dysfunction, whereas SCID-II-PQ PD scales (Mdn ⫽ .24) and NEOPI-3FH (Mdn ⫽ .20) facets were less severe. This finding supports Hypothesis 3a that pathological traits will relate more strongly to severe problems than normal-range traits. Median amplitude values indicated that measures included a similar proportion, and considerable number, of scales related to specific interpersonal problems. To understand these relations, SSM Parameters for individual scales with amplitudes greater than or equal to .10 and R2 greater than or equal to .70 are organized by IIP-SC octant and displacement degree in Table 4. The remaining scales yielded less meaningful angular displacements and thus are provided separately in Table 5. Examining Table 4 informs the prediction (H1) that previous findings with the DSM–5 PD models would replicate. Eight (of 10; 83%) SCID-II-PQ scales related to the IIP-SC with specificity (amplitude ⬎ .10); the highest amplitudes PDs related to Vindictive (Narcissistic, Antisocial, Paranoid PDs), Cold (Schizoid PD), and Socially Avoidant (Avoidant PD) interpersonal problems. Histrionic PD was the only PD related to warm interpersonal problems (i.e., Intrusive); Dependent PD (DPD) did not show its predicted relation to warm problems. The 17 (of 25; 68%) PID-5 facets with specific relations to the IIP-SC mostly related to Domineering (e.g., Manipulativeness), Vindictive (e.g., Callousness), and Cold (e.g., Restricted Affectivity) interpersonal problems; few traits related to warmer or purely submissive problems. To test the hypothesis (H2) that Extraversion and Agreeableness would relate to interpersonal problems most specifically, facets of

19

the PID-5 and NEO-PI-3FH were examined based on domain. As Tables 3 and 4 indicate, NEO Extraversion and Agreeableness facets all related to specific problems, whereas the other domains lacked such uniformity. PID-5 antagonism and detachment facets (variants of Extraversion and Agreeableness) were similarly specific; all antagonism facets and all but one detachment facet related to specific problems. Predictions regarding interpersonal coverage differences between trait models (H3b & H3c) were examined by analyzing Table 4 on a measure-by-measure basis. As noted above, the PID-5 facets were related mostly to Domineering, Vindictive, and Cold problems. The CAT-PD-SF traits showed a similar pattern of relations with IIP-SC, as the 22 (of 33; 67%) facets with specific relations to the IIP-SC were mostly in the Domineering (e.g., Norm Violation), Vindictive (e.g., Hostile Aggression), and Cold (e.g., Relationship Insecurity) problems octants. The CAT-PD-SF provided some additional coverage of warm problems, as Rudeness related to somewhat warm problems (i.e., angular displacement ⫽ 85.2°). The CAT-PD-SF also provided some additional coverage of the Socially Avoidant octant, with two traits having large amplitudes (Emotional Detachment and Social Withdrawal), relative to the one PID-5 facet (Withdrawal) with very specific relations. However, overall, the PID-5 and CAT-PD-SF traits converged quite strongly in their relations to the IIP-SC. This is in line with the prediction (H3) that more dramatic differences would exist between models of differing measurement range. Finally, the NEO-PI-3FH showed a different pattern of relations to the IIP-SC. Of the 24 facets (of 30; 80%) with specific relations to the IIP-SC, the most specific were located within the Nonassertive (e.g., Modesty), Socially Avoidant (e.g., -Gregariousness), Cold (e.g., -Warmth) and Vindictive (e.g., -Altruism) octants. Only one facet (-Straightforwardness) related to Domineering problems with moderate specificity (amplitude ⬎ .20), whereas several NEO-PI-3FH traits related to pure submissiveness (e.g., Nonassertive problems). Similar to all measures, however, the NEO-PI3FH did not relate strongly or specifically to warm problems. The NEO-PI-3FH facet Feelings (amplitude ⫽ .17) was the most specific; however, it did not relate to interpersonal distress (elevation ⫽ .03 [⫺.04, .10]). The trait of -Order was less specific in its relation to warm problems (amplitude ⫽ .14), though its elevation was slightly higher (.09). Finally, -Deliberation and Anxiety also related to warm problems, but were more strongly characterized by submissiveness and dominance than warmth.

Discussion The present study compared four PD measures by analyzing their relations with interpersonal dysfunction in a psychiatric sample. In doing so, previous findings relating the DSM–5 PD measures to interpersonal problems largely were replicated. Across trait measures, Agreeableness and Extraversion facets related most specifically to interpersonal problems; however, PID-5 and CAT-PD traits both related to generally greater problem severity than did NEO-PI-3FH traits. In addition, pathological traits more strongly related to dominance problems than did normal-range traits, which more strongly covered submissive problems.

WILLIAMS AND SIMMS

20

Table 2 Personality Pathology Scale Descriptive Statistics and Correlations With IIP-SC Octant Scales ␣

M

SD

PA

BC

DE

FG

HI

JK

LM

NO

NEO-PI-3FH Neuroticism Anxiety Angry hostility Depression Self-consciousness Impulsiveness Vulnerability Extraversion Warmth Gregariousness Assertiveness Activity Excitement-seeking Positive emotions Agreeableness Trust Straightforwardness Altruism Compliance Modesty Tender-mindedness Conscientiousness Competence Order Dutifulness Achievement striving Self-discipline Deliberation Openness Fantasy Aesthetics Feelings Actions Ideas Values M (facets) SD (facets) M (re-keyed facets) SD (re-keyed facets)

.90 .67 .75 .77 .62 .56 .68 .83 .62 .75 .70 .41 .40 .67 .82 .75 .66 .63 .59 .56 .60 .88 .56 .75 .50 .73 .80 .66 .80 .54 .66 .49 .43 .62 .40 .62 .11 — —

74.7 14.2 10.9 13.8 12.2 11.9 11.6 75.1 14.7 10.7 11.6 11.9 12.9 13.2 84.9 12.3 14.4 16.7 12.0 13.7 15.9 78.5 14.0 13.7 12.9 13.6 13.2 11.1 84.0 12.9 14.4 14.8 12.5 14.0 15.5 13.24 1.48 — —

17.4 3.6 4.0 4.3 3.7 3.4 3.7 15.1 3.6 4.1 3.9 3.2 3.7 3.9 12.9 3.8 3.7 2.8 3.6 3.3 3.0 15.6 3.1 3.8 3.3 3.6 4.1 3.4 13.1 3.4 3.8 3.1 2.9 3.7 2.8 3.56 .39 — —

.39 .16 .54 .18 .24 .31 .33 ⫺.13 ⫺.32 ⫺.09 .12 ⫺.04 .02 ⫺.21 ⫺.52 ⫺.33 ⫺.40 ⫺.42 ⫺.44 ⫺.15 ⫺.24 ⫺.29 ⫺.31 ⫺.01 ⫺.29 ⴚ.18 ⫺.23 ⫺.28 ⫺.21 ⫺.05 ⫺.16 ⫺.05 ⫺.18 ⫺.17 ⫺.23 ⫺.10 .24 .20 .17

.41 .23 .47 .26 .34 .23 .35 ⫺.35 ⫺.52 ⫺.27 ⫺.07 ⫺.15 .00 ⫺.39 ⫺.53 ⫺.53 ⫺.31 ⫺.48 ⫺.42 ⫺.03 ⫺.25 ⴚ.25 ⫺.32 .08 ⫺.27 ⫺.25 ⴚ.17 ⫺.21 ⫺.29 ⫺.13 ⫺.26 ⫺.08 ⫺.28 ⫺.20 ⫺.22 ⫺.13 .27 .25 .16

.37 .23 .25 .33 .41 .15 .31 ⫺.54 ⫺.61 ⫺.43 ⫺.25 ⫺.27 ⫺.13 ⫺.48 ⫺.26 ⫺.38 ⫺.11 ⫺.35 ⫺.21 .20 ⫺.11 ⫺.26 ⫺.34 .04 ⫺.20 ⫺.32 ⫺.23 ⫺.13 ⫺.24 ⫺.19 ⫺.17 ⫺.15 ⫺.21 ⫺.13 ⫺.12 ⫺.12 .25 .24 .15

.48 .38 .24 .41 .59 .16 .40 ⫺.73 ⫺.62 ⫺.61 ⫺.52 ⫺.38 ⫺.27 ⫺.52 ⫺.07 ⫺.31 .07 ⫺.25 ⫺.09 .38 ⫺.09 ⫺.31 ⫺.36 ⫺.07 ⫺.17 ⫺.37 ⫺.33 ⫺.08 ⫺.20 ⫺.09 ⫺.18 ⫺.10 ⫺.29 ⫺.12 .01 ⫺.11 .32 .27 .20

.43 .44 .04 .35 .53 .21 .41 ⫺.50 ⫺.26 ⫺.31 ⫺.52 ⫺.29 ⫺.33 ⫺.28 .14 ⫺.07 .11 ⫺.04 .20 .32 .01 ⫺.36 ⫺.37 ⫺.23 ⫺.10 ⫺.42 ⫺.42 ⴚ.03 ⫺.05 ⫺.03 ⫺.08 .09 ⫺.18 ⫺.10 .14 ⫺.04 .28 .20 .20

.42 .42 .05 .35 .48 .24 .39 ⫺.37 ⫺.18 ⫺.18 ⫺.42 ⫺.23 ⫺.21 ⫺.27 .15 ⫺.11 .09 .05 .20 .29 .06 ⫺.34 ⫺.38 ⫺.21 ⫺.08 ⫺.38 ⫺.35 ⫺.09 ⫺.03 ⫺.03 ⫺.06 .12 ⫺.13 ⫺.10 .10 ⫺.02 .25 .17 .18

.39 .40 .11 .33 .35 .22 .37 ⫺.25 ⫺.02 ⫺.13 ⫺.26 ⫺.21 ⫺.21 ⫺.19 .21 ⫺.04 .07 .21 .13 .29 .19 ⫺.26 ⴚ.30 ⫺.13 ⴚ.06 ⫺.25 ⫺.29 ⫺.12 .03 ⫺.04 .05 .20 ⫺.10 ⫺.07 .11 .02 .21 .13 .17

.36 .22 .30 .18 .23 .36 .34 .02 .04 .10 .05 ⫺.07 ⫺.04 ⴚ.04 ⫺.16 ⴚ.05 ⫺.17 ⫺.07 ⫺.11 ⫺.19 .02 ⫺.38 ⫺.31 ⫺.22 ⫺.26 ⫺.24 ⫺.32 ⫺.33 .05 .14 ⫺.02 .18 ⴚ.03 ⴚ.05 ⫺.02 ⫺.01 .20 .12 .15

PID-5 Negative affect Submissiveness Separation insecurity Anxiousness Emotional lability Perseveration Detachment Depressivity Suspiciousness Restricted affect Withdrawal Intimacy-avoidance Anhedonia Antagonism Manipulativeness Deceitfulness Hostility Callousness Attention seeking Grandiosity Disinhibition Irresponsibility Impulsivity

.94 .78 .87 .91 .91 .87 .93 .94 .83 .75 .91 .86 .90 .92 .84 .87 .88 .89 .91 .78 .93 .81 .86

1.3 1.3 1.0 1.5 1.4 1.1 1.1 0.9 1.2 0.9 1.2 0.7 1.2 0.7 0.8 0.6 1.1 0.4 0.9 0.7 1.0 0.7 1.0

0.7 0.7 0.8 0.8 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.7 0.7 0.6 0.7 0.7 0.8 0.5 0.7 0.6 0.7 0.5 0.7 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.7

.46 .12 .39 .37 .43 .42 .34 .38 .44 .26 .30 .19 .31 .48 .41 .46 .66 .58 .41 .38 .52 .45 .50

.49 .17 .38 .47 .42 .42 .52 .48 .61 .39 .51 .30 .44 .44 .32 .47 .66 .65 .21 .37 .50 .44 .46

.40 .17 .26 .43 .35 .40 .74 .54 .48 .53 .70 .53 .55 .27 .19 .28 .46 .49 .04 .24 .46 .40 .36

.46 .29 .32 .48 .41 .44 .68 .54 .38 .35 .73 .33 .57 .04 ⫺.04 .11 .37 .27 ⫺.17 .05 .40 .30 .25

.41 .56 .25 .44 .38 .44 .44 .47 .14 .16 .39 .23 .42 .07 .03 .14 .14 .09 .01 .02 .37 .29 .16

.46 .60 .31 .46 .42 .46 .39 .50 .24 .15 .31 .21 .40 .09 .05 .14 .15 .12 .07 .04 .42 .32 .24

.47 .51 .31 .45 .47 .45 .29 .44 .25 .01 .21 .10 .37 .05 .03 .07 .13 .03 .08 .03 .35 .23 .22

.43 .31 .40 .32 .41 .39 .12 .33 .19 ⫺.03 ⫺.01 .06 .22 .33 .28 .30 .31 .20 .51 .27 .47 .40 .41

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Measure/scale name

PERSONALITY DISORDER AND INTERPERSONAL DYSFUNCTION

21

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Table 2 (continued) Measure/scale name



M

SD

PA

BC

Distractibility Rigid perfectionism Risk taking Psychoticism Eccentricity Perceptual-dysregulation Unusual beliefs M (facets) SD (facets)

.91 .91 .87 .96 .96 .87 .83 .87 .05

1.2 1.3 1.2 0.8 1.0 0.7 0.6 1.00 .28

0.8 0.7 0.6 0.6 0.8 0.6 0.6 .69 .09

.38 .38 .26 .50 .45 .50 .38 .39 .11

.38 .42 .22 .48 .45 .50 .34 .42 .12

CAT-PD-SF Affective lability Anger Anhedonia Anxiousness Callousness Cognitive problems Depressiveness Domineering Emotional detachment Exhibitionism Fantasy proneness Grandiosity Health anxiety Hostile aggression Irresponsibility Manipulativeness Mistrust Non-perseverance Non-planfulness Norm violation Peculiarity Perfectionism Relationship-insecurity Rigidity Risk taking Romantic disinterest Rudeness Self harm Social withdrawal Submissiveness Unusual beliefs Unusual experiences Workaholism M SD

.86 .85 .89 .85 .84 .88 .88 .84 .86 .83 .83 .82 .84 .87 .86 .85 .88 .88 .84 .84 .82 .86 .83 .81 .84 .88 .80 .86 .87 .86 .85 .82 .86 .85 .02

2.8 2.7 2.5 3.0 1.6 2.6 2.7 2.2 2.9 2.2 2.6 1.9 2.6 1.5 2.0 1.6 2.8 2.5 2.5 2.3 2.5 3.3 2.5 2.6 1.8 2.2 2.1 1.6 2.8 2.3 1.5 1.6 2.5 2.33 .48

1.1 1.1 1.0 1.0 0.7 1.0 1.0 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.8 1.0 0.7 0.8 0.7 1.1 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.8 0.9 1.0 0.9 0.9 1.0 1.0 0.7 0.8 0.9 .92 .12

.50 .54 .35 .26 .54 .28 .29 .70 .15 .26 .41 .39 .37 .64 .23 .37 .47 .42 .36 .46 .45 .08 .45 .43 .22 .00 .63 .30 .20 .20 .31 .46 .09 .36 .16

SCID-II PQ Avoidant PD Dependent PD Obsessive Compulsive PD Paranoid PD Schizotypal PD Schizoid PD Histrionic PD Narcissistic PD Borderline PD Antisocial PD/Conduct Dis. M SD

.73 .60 .54 .78 .72 .51 .51 .78 .86 .81 .68 .13

2.1 1.8 1.9 2.5 2.5 1.5 1.8 3.6 4.2 2.6 2.45 .84

.20 .28 .27 .41 .33 .19 .30 .46 .46 .32 .32 .10

10.4 10.2 13.5 12.0 14.9 8.2 8.8 22.2 21.6 16.2 13.8 5.0

DE

FG

HI

JK

LM

NO

.41 .37 .09 .46 .43 .45 .33 .38 .15

.45 .33 ⫺.09 .35 .39 .35 .18 .30 .21

.47 .17 ⫺.12 .24 .28 .26 .08 .24 .18

.49 .22 ⫺.04 .31 .35 .32 .14 .27 .17

.42 .24 ⫺.04 .30 .31 .30 .19 .23 .17

.39 .16 .19 .40 .40 .37 .28 .28 .13

.56 .51 .45 .40 .54 .34 .42 .42 .23 .09 .41 .36 .39 .69 .18 .47 .65 .41 .34 .41 .43 .15 .58 .34 .26 .10 .44 .23 .39 .32 .27 .46 .14 .37 .15

.47 .36 .49 .36 .43 .43 .41 .31 .55 ⫺.08 .43 .16 .39 .43 .10 .36 .49 .38 .29 .19 .44 .13 .50 .25 .18 .25 .27 .21 .61 .29 .28 .44 .18 .33 .15

.45 .38 .54 .42 .26 .47 .48 .09 .50 ⫺.37 .37 .05 .37 .28 .15 .19 .37 .47 .15 .08 .36 .10 .49 .22 .04 .16 .19 .25 .79 .36 .07 .22 .08 .27 .21

.39 .00 .31 .40 .14 .48 .29 ⫺.04 .26 ⫺.19 .30 .01 .26 ⫺.03 .23 .22 .19 .42 .16 ⫺.15 .35 ⫺.04 .34 .07 .00 .07 .06 .17 .49 .56 .09 .16 .05 .18 .19

.40 .09 .26 .41 .10 .51 .25 ⫺.08 .25 ⫺.16 .35 ⫺.02 .29 .12 .18 .14 .23 .42 .18 ⫺.07 .42 ⫺.02 .34 .06 .03 .03 .18 .25 .37 .66 .14 .23 .11 .20 .18

.38 .20 .29 .42 ⫺.12 .51 .29 ⫺.02 .11 ⫺.09 .40 ⫺.07 .39 .11 .00 ⫺.01 .21 .32 .20 .01 .43 .14 .26 .20 ⫺.04 ⫺.12 .29 .21 .23 .52 .10 .26 .13 .19 .18

.34 .25 .32 .29 .12 .33 .30 .29 ⫺.24 .37 .39 .33 .31 .28 .22 .16 .17 .34 .32 .28 .36 .13 .17 .32 .09 ⫺.09 .54 .19 ⫺.08 .37 .26 .35 .10 .24 .16

.35 .32 .19 .55 .36 .35 .16 .41 .51 .32 .35 .12

.49 .29 .26 .49 .36 .46 ⫺.02 .27 .45 .22 .33 .16

.68 .31 .21 .41 .33 .35 ⫺.17 .11 .42 .10 .28 .23

.47 .30 .10 .16 .16 .08 ⫺.06 ⫺.03 .25 ⫺.03 .14 .17

.39 .35 .09 .16 .20 .09 .02 .02 .27 ⫺.04 .15 .15

.31 .33 .10 .17 .20 .01 .06 .01 .30 ⫺.01 .15 .13

.02 .28 .08 .19 .20 .03 .38 .28 .31 .02 .18 .13

Note. PA ⫽ Domineering (90°); BC ⫽ Vindictive (135°); DE ⫽ Cold (180°); FG ⫽ Socially Avoidant (225°); HI ⫽ Nonassertive (270°); JK ⫽ Exploitable (315°); LM ⫽ Overly Nurturant (0/360°); NO ⫽ Intrusive (45°; Soldz et al., 1995). Bold values represent the largest correlation in a row. For personality traits, domain-level scales are left-justified relative to facets. For all measures, r ⬎ |.15| is significant at p ⬍ .05 and r ⬎ |.25| significant at p ⬍ .01. Lowest pair-wise N by measure: CAT-PD-SF, N ⫽ 136; PID-5, N ⫽ 437; NEO-PI-3FH, N ⫽ 381; SCID-II PQ, N ⫽ 433.

WILLIAMS AND SIMMS

22

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Table 3 Median SSM Parameters by Measure Scale

Elevation

Amplitude

R2

SCID-II PQ CAT-PD-SF PID-5 facets PID-5 domains NEO-PI-3FH facets NEO-PI-3FH domains NEO-PI-3FH facets (re-keyed) NEO-PI-3FH domains (re-keyed)

.24 (.09, .37) .29 (⫺.02, .44) .34 (.06, .46) .43 (.22, .45) ⫺.11 (⫺.33, .40) ⫺.13 (⫺.36, .35) .20 (.03, .40) .31 (.12, .40)

.17 (.02, .25) .15 (.02, .34) .16 (.01, .31) .12 (.01, .26) .16 (.03, .33) .16 (.04, .37) .16 (.03, .33) .17 (.04, .37)

.89 (.28, .95) .82 (.03, .97) .83 (.47, .95) .88 (.13, .93) .88 (.59, .98) .87 (.31, .93) .88 (.59, .98) .87 (.31, .93)

Note. Median values for each parameter are provided, along with the range for the parameter within parentheses. Elevations and amplitudes ⫽ .10 are small, .20 moderate, .30 large. R2 ⬎ .70 are acceptable.

DSM–5 Section II and III PD Measures Recent statistical advances (e.g., Zimmerman & Wright, 2015) and a patient sample were used to reexamine previous research relating DSM–5 PD models to the IIP (e.g., Soldz et al., 1993; Wright et al., 2012). Previous findings with DSM–5 traditional categorical PDs (i.e., Section II) generally were replicated, the exception being that dependent PD (DPD) did not relate to specific (i.e., warm) interpersonal problems, as indicated by DPD’s low amplitude value (i.e., ⬍ .01). The low amplitude value was driven by DPD’s correlations with Domineering, Vindictive, and Cold problems, which were higher than expected. Despite the low amplitude value, DPD did relate to severe interpersonal distress, as indicated by the elevation parameter (i.e., .31). Possible explanations for these findings include (a) differences between the SCID-II PQ and previously used measures, (b) failure of previous studies to quantify specificity (i.e., amplitude), and (c) random sampling error. Disentangling the influences of these factors is difficult; however, the low internal consistency of SCID-II PQ DPD scale relative to measures used in previous studies (e.g., Pincus & Wiggins, 1990) is a plausible explanation for this divergent finding. In general, the present findings for the emerging trait model (i.e., Section III) mirror those from Wright et al.’s (2012) student sample. Exceptions were Rigid Perfectionism and Suspiciousness relating to more specific problems and Anhedonia and Irresponsibility relating to less specific ones (i.e., differences in amplitude). Notably, these facet-level differences were small; in contrast, domain-level discrepancies were larger. Disinhibition and Negative Affect related to relatively nonspecific problems, in contrast with their specificity in Wright et al. Differences in scoring methods may have produced this finding. The present study averaged three traits within each domain and specifically excluded interstitial traits of potential interpersonal relevance (e.g., submissiveness; APA, 2013), whereas Wright et al. used factor scores that were based on all traits. Overall, Wright et al.’s (2012) results generally replicated in our patient sample, but more work is needed to replicate these findings given the variety of PID-5 domain scoring methods used in the literature.

Extraversion and Agreeableness As predicted from previous research (e.g., McCrae & Costa, 1989), Extraversion and Agreeableness facets related to the most specific interpersonal problems. Interestingly, NEO-PI-3FH Angry

Hostility (Neuroticism) and PID-5 Submissiveness (Negative Affect) also related to quite specific problems (amplitude ⬎ .20). Weak loadings on posited domains or sizable loadings on other domains may explain this finding (e.g., McCrae et al., 2005; Wright & Simms, 2014). Several NEO Neuroticism facets also had somewhat specific (amplitude ⬎ .10) relations to Nonassertive problems. This may be the result of the NEO Neuroticism domain containing most of the measure’s negatively valenced content, relative to pathological trait models (e.g., Wright & Simms, 2014). Despite some informative discrepancies, the confirmation of this hypothesis highlights a point of agreement between normal-range and pathological trait models.

Differences Between Normal-Range and Pathological Measures Hypotheses regarding measurement-range were broadly supported, in that all pathological measures related to somewhat different and more severe problems (i.e., higher elevation) than the NEO-PI-3FH. Notably, these conclusions held regardless of whether the NEO-PI-3FH traits were keyed normally or keyed to maximize relations with the IIP-SC. However, the prediction that normal-range and pathological traits would be differentiated by coverage of warm versus cold problems was not supported. Evaluating this hypothesis is complex, in that NEO-PI-3FH traits were re-keyed. Correlational results suggest that many of these scales, if not re-keyed, would have positive amplitudes, large negative elevations, and angular displacements indicating warmth. However, large negative elevations imply that these scales relate to warm interpersonal behaviors rather than problems (e.g., Hopwood et al., 2013). Re-keyed NEO facets did not cover warm problems better than pathological traits, instead relating to cold problems with similar strength as pathological traits. In this sense, pathological and normal-range traits converged in their (lacking) coverage of warm interpersonal problems. It is notable that the present study provided representation of all currently prominent PD models and thus a broad array of PD constructs, yet still found limited coverage of warm problems. Several possible explanations for this limited coverage exist. First, warm behavior may largely be adaptive rather than impairing (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995); it might be uncommon for warm behaviors to lead to problems. If few people have warm problems, then fewer relations to common personality pathology might be expected. Second, linguistic constraints could explain the relative

PERSONALITY DISORDER AND INTERPERSONAL DYSFUNCTION

23

Table 4 SSM Parameters for PD and PD Trait Scales Related to Specific Interpersonal Problems

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Angular displacement PA (67.5°–112.5°): Domineering 73.8 (54.3, 91.4) 80.5 (53.5, 106.7) 85.2 (59.4, 104) 98.4 (78.9, 115.1) 98.8 (82.8, 114.1) 101.8 (39, 143.8) 105.8 (74.8, 141.5) 106.7 (63.4, 139.4) 106.8 (88.5, 124.3) 107 (92.7, 119.2) 107.4 (94.7, 119.8) 109.1 (84.8, 131) 110.1 (88.3, 130.9) 110.7 (93.5, 124.3) BC (112.5°–157.5°): Vindictive 112.8 (91.1, 133.7) 113.8 (97.2, 128.5) 114 (77.1, 139.9) 114.1 (98.7, 127.2) 115.3 (101.5, 128.8) 115.7 (103.2, 126.6) 117.9 (80.6, 145.9) 120.9 (95.3, 143.8) 123.4 (108.3, 138) 124.5 (107.2, 141.2) 124.7 (115.3, 133.1) 125.1 (101.6, 148.7) 125.8 (96, 148.5) 126.4 (118.7, 134.2) 130.1 (89.8, 170.8) 131.1 (122.2, 139.5) 135 (122.2, 148) 137.4 (130.3, 144.8) 143.2 (124.6, 164.3) 145.9 (130.3, 161.4) 146.9 (127.8, 165) 147.4 (132.1, 165.6) 149 (126.2, 170.8) 149 (140.2, 158.3) 150 (127.9, 180.4) 150.3 (127, 182.5) 151.9 (123.9, 181) 154 (141.1, 167.3) 155.6 (140.4, 170.3) DE (157.5°–202.5°): Cold 165 (145.1, 184) 168.1 (139.3, 197.9) 175.5 (144.4, 205.7) 175.7 (161.6, 192.1) 176.2 (139.9, 230.7) 180.2 (168, 193.4) 187.5 (177.9, 197.7) 190.6 (152.3, 231.6) 191.2 (157.3, 231.9) 194.4 (175.4, 212.3) 198.6 (162.6, 236.8) FG (202.5°–247.5°): Socially avoidant 202.7 (192.9, 213.2) 203.5 (192.4, 214.6) 203.6 (186.3, 221.5) 214.1 (193.5, 233.2) 215.7 (192.7, 241) 217 (203.6, 229.5)

Measure

Scale

Elevation

Amplitude

R2

⫺.02 (⫺.13, .11) .16 (.09, .22) .33 (.20, .44) .06 (⫺.01, .13) .16 (.09, .23) .25 (.14, .35) .23 (.14, .33 .19 (.00, .34) .33 (.27, .38) .22 (.14, .29) .08 (.01, .15) .15 (.02, .28) .15 (.02, .28) .17 (.09, .24)

.30 (.22, .39) .13 (.08, .18) .25 (.17, .34) .19 (.13, .25) .20 (.15, .25) .11 (.04, .20) .17 (.07, .27) .12 (.06, .19) .16 (.11, 21) .23 (.18, .28) .25 (.20, .31) .29 (.20, .39) .23 (.13, .33) .20 (.15, .26)

.85 .89 .95 .94 .86 .93 .92 .80 .93 .88 .93 .97 .84 .94

CAT-PD-SF NEO-PI-3FH CAT-PD-SF PID-5 PID-5 CAT-PD-SF CAT-PD-SF CAT-PD-SF PID-5 PID-5 NEO-PI-3FH CAT-PD-SF CAT-PD-SF PID-5

Exhibitionism C: -Deliberation Rudeness Dis: Risk taking Ant: Manipulativeness Non-planfulness Rigidity Unusual beliefs Dis: Impulsivity Antagonism A: -Straightforwardness Norm violation Grandiosity Ant: Grandiosity

PID-5 CAT-PD-SF CAT-PD-SF PID-5 NEO-PI-3FH SCID-II PQ NEO-PI-3FH PID-5 CAT-PD-SF NEO-PI-3FH NEO-PI-3FH PID-5 CAT-PD-SF NEO-PI-3FH CAT-PD-SF PID-5 SCID-II PQ PID-5 SCID-II PQ PID-5 NEO-PI-3FH CAT-PD-SF CAT-PD-SF NEO-PI-3FH CAT-PD-SF PID-5 SCID-II PQ SCID-II PQ NEO-PI-3FH

Psy: Unusual beliefs and experiences Domineering Unusual experiences Ant: Deceitfulness N: Angry hostility Narcissistic PD C: -Dutifulness Psychoticism Hostile Aggression O: -Values A: -Compliance Psy: Perceptual dysregulation Anger -Agreeableness Risk taking Ant: Hostility Antisocial PD (childhood criteria) Ant: Callousness Borderline PD Det: Suspiciousness A: -Tenderness Callousness Mistrust A: -Altruism Manipulativeness Dis: Rigid perfectionism Schizotypal PD Paranoid PD A: -Trust

.24 (.17, .31) .21 (.10, .30) .32 (.16, .46) .25 (.17, .32) .25 (.19, .31) .19 (.13, .25) .18 (.11, .24) .38 (.31, .44) .32 (.19, .42) .03 (⫺.04, .10) .09 (.03, .16) .38 (.31, .44) .29 (.19, .39) .13 (.07, .19) .10 (⫺.01, .20) .36 (.30, .42) .11 (.04, .19) .30 (.23, .38) .37 (.31, .42) .34 (.28, .40) .05 (⫺.01, .11) .25 (.12, .36) .35 (.25, .43) .17 (.10, .24) .24 (.10, .35) .28 (.22, .34) .27 (.20, .33) .32 (.26, .37) .23 (.16, .29)

.14 (.09, .19) .34 (.25, .43) .15 (.07, .24) .19 (.14, .24) .23 (.18, .29) .25 (.20, .30) .11 (.06, .17) .12 (.07, .17) .32 (.24, .41) .19 (.14, .24) .33 (.28, .39) .12 (.07, .17) .23 (.14, .34) .37 (.32, .43) .14 (.05, .24) .28 (.23, .33) .19 (.14, .25) .30 (.26, .35) .13 (.08, .18) .19 (.13, .25) .18 (.12, .24) .29 (.20, .37) .21 (.13, .30) .31 (.25, .37) .18 (.09, .28) .11 (.05, .18) .10 (.06, .16) .21 (.16, .26 .22 (.17, .28)

.95 .89 .92 .83 .90 .95 .84 .93 .91 .94 .96 .91 .82 .93 .87 .92 .90 .91 .89 .78 .79 .84 .81 .91 .80 .73 .85 .93 .86

NEO-PI-3FH NEO-PI-3FH CAT-PD-SF SCID-II PQ CAT-PD-SF PID-5 NEO-PI-3FH NEO-PI-3FH NEO-PI-3FH PID-5 CAT-PD-SF

-Openness O: -Aesthetics Relationship insecurity Schizoid PD Anhedonia Det: Restricted affectivity E: -Warmth O: -Fantasy E: -Actions Det: Intimacy avoidance Romantic disinterest

.12 (.04, .19) .11 (.04, .17) .39 (.28, .49) .20 (.13, .26) .38 (.27, .47) .23 (.15, .30) .31 (.25, .37) .05 (⫺.01, .11) .18 (.10, .25) .24 (.17, .31) .05 (⫺.07, .16)

.17 (.11, .22) .12 (.06, .19) .16 (.09, .24) .22 (.17, .27) .12 (.04, .22) .24 (.19, .30) .33 (.27, .38) .10 (.04, .16) .10 (.05, .17) .18 (.13, .23) .16 (.07, .25)

.87 .84 .76 .92 .82 .89 .93 .59 .76 .82 .92

PID-5 PID-5 NEO-PI-3FH CAT-PD-SF PID-5 NEO-PI-3FH

Det: Withdrawal Detachment E: -Positive emotions Emotional detachment Det: Anhedonia E: -Gregariousness

.39 (.34, .44) .44 (.39, .49) .30 (.24, .36) .22 (.12, .33) .41 (.36, .46) .24 (.18, .30)

.31 (.27, .36) .90 .26 (.22, .30) .89 .20 (.15, .26) .86 .29 (.22, .37) .79 .14 (.09, .19) .83 .27 (.23, .32) .89 (table continues)

WILLIAMS AND SIMMS

24 Table 4 (continued)

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Angular displacement 221.7 (206.5, 237.2) 228.7 (217.6, 239.3) 231.2 (219.1, 242.8) 246.7 (221.2, 271.8) HI (247.5°–292.5°): Nonassertive 252.3 (234.5, 268.8) 256.8 (222.4, 286.8) 260.4 (226.5, 290.4) 263.5 (252.6, 273.8) 265 (252.1, 277) 275.4 (255.2, 294.2) 283.5 (223.4, 321.8) 291.2 (269.5, 310.9) JK (292.5°–337.5°): Exploitable 308.2 (286.9, 330.8) 311.8 (300.5, 323.3) 326.2 (301.5, 348.6) LM (337.5°–22.5°): Overly nurturant 353.3 (335.8, 13.3) NO (22.5°–67.5°): Intrusive 66 (52.3, 79.9) 66.8 (54.5, 79.2)

Measure

Scale

Elevation

Amplitude

R2

CAT-PD-SF NEO-PI-3FH SCID-II PQ NEO-PI-3FH

Social withdrawal -Extraversion Avoidant PD E: -Activity

.37 (.27, .48) .36 (.30, .41) .36 (.31, .42) .21 (.15, .26)

.34 (.27, .42) .30 (.26, .35) .25 (.21, .29) .14 (.10, .20)

.91 .93 .87 .90

NEO-PI-3FH NEO-PI-3FH NEO-PI-3FH NEO-PI-3FH NEO-PI-3FH NEO-PI-3FH CAT-PD-SF NEO-PI-3FH

N: Self-consciousness N: Depression C: -Achievement-striving E: -Assertiveness A: Modesty E: -Excitement-seeking Cognitive problems N: Anxiety

.40 (.34, .45) .30 (.23, .35) .30 (.24, .36) .23 (.17, .29) .14 (.07, .21) .15 (.08, .21) .42 (.33, .50) .31 (.25, .37)

.17 (.12, .22) .10 (.06, .15) .11 (.06, .16) .32 (.28, .37) .27 (.23, .32) .17 (.11, .23) .11 (.04, .20) .14 (.10, .19)

.96 .84 .30 .98 .84 .94 .82 .92

CAT-PD-SF PID-5 NEO-PI-3FH

Submissiveness Na: Submissiveness C: -Order

.41 (.30, .50) .34 (.28, .40) .09 (.03, .16)

.19 (.11, 29) .25 (.20, .31) .14 (.08, .21)

.86 .95 .80

NEO-PI-3FH

O: Feelings

.03 (⫺.04, .10)

.17 (.12, .23)

.92

SCID-II PQ PID-5

Histrionic PD Ant: Attention-seeking

.09 (.02, .15) .14 (.08, .21)

.23 (.19, .28) .27 (.23, .31)

.89 .83

Note. Scales with amplitudes ⬍ .10 or R2 ⬍ .70 were omitted, as placing them within one octant is problematic. N ⫽ Neuroticism; E ⫽ Extraversion; O ⫽ Openness; A ⫽ Agreeableness; C ⫽ Conscientiousness; Na ⫽ Negative Affect; Det ⫽ Detachment; Ant ⫽ Antagonism; Dis ⫽ Disinhibition; Psy ⫽ Psychoticism. Elevations and amplitudes ⫽ .10 are small, .20 moderate, .30 large. R2 ⬎ .70 are acceptable. In all cases the probability of confidence interval accuracy was above .70. Scales representing higher-order domains are in bold.

dearth of warm pathology across PD models, as there are fewer negatively valenced descriptors for warm behaviors (Leary, 1957; Coker, Samuel, & Widiger, 2002). Thus it might be the case that either (a) PD trait models fail to represent reality because they were founded on lexical work (e.g., Block, 1995), or (b) the IIP-SC overemphasizes warm problems. However, linguistic underrepresentation or cultural minimization of warm pathology does not equate to it being nonexistent or clinically irrelevant (e.g., Block, 1995). Finally, people may be less aware of their warm interpersonal problems. Supporting this possibility is research suggesting that individuals with PD bother others most when they attempt to engage in warm, affiliative behaviors (e.g., Williams et al., 2014). More vexing is the finding that more normal-range traits (NEOPI-3FH) related to submissive problems than did pathological traits, which related in greater number to dominant problems. If the facets within these measures are taken to represent distinct patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors that lead to or include interpersonal problems, then this finding may be interpreted as measures differing in their ability to describe diverse pathways to, or contexts for, dominant versus submissive problems. In other words, substantive differences in the constructs represented by these measures may explain this finding. For instance, no NEO trait appears to correspond to grandiosity in interpersonal content (i.e., domineering), whereas grandiosity is among a number of CAT-PD and PID-5 traits related to domineering problems. In contrast, the NEO traits better cover specifically nonassertive problems; Modesty and -Assertiveness both related to problems within this octant and lacked CAT-PD or PID-5 counterparts.

Implications The CAT-PD, PID-5, and NEO-PI-3FH trait measures of PD correspond relatively well at the domain level (e.g., Wright & Simms, 2014); however, the present study suggests potential facetlevel differences in constructs. Such discrepancies in PD models could impede the ability to translate research findings across models. In addition, these models may yield nonequivalent clinical descriptions. For instance, in the PID-5 and CAT-PD trait measures, there are numerous ways to describe problematically dominant individuals, whereas the NEO facets provide fewer options. Conversely, the pathological trait models do not describe clients that struggle to assert themselves as well as the NEO. These differences have considerable clinical importance when one considers traits in terms of social– cognitive processes (e.g., Fleeson & Jayawickreme, 2015). From this perspective, pathological personality traits describe interrelated, clinically relevant social– cognitive processes. Interpersonal theory provides a framework for connecting traits to processes by coordinating multiple levels of interpersonal phenomena (e.g., problematic behaviors, desired outcomes, sensitivities; Hopwood et al., 2011) across time (e.g., Locke & Sadler, 2007). For instance, grandiose individuals may be prone to interpret dominant behavior in others as cold or threatening, relative to less grandiose individuals (Roche, Pincus, Hyde, Conroy, & Ram, 2013). Explicating personality pathology in such terms may inform a variety of therapy approaches by suggesting interpersonal schemas (e.g., Safran, 1990), objectrelations (e.g., Lukowitsky & Pincus, 2011), and avoidant coping strategies (e.g., Antony & Roemer, 2003) that may serve as treatment targets. The present results suggest that prominent PD mod-

PERSONALITY DISORDER AND INTERPERSONAL DYSFUNCTION

25

Table 5 SSM Parameters for PD and PD Trait Scales Not Related to Specific Interpersonal Problems Measure SCID-II PQ

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

CAT-PD-SF

PID-5

NEO-PI-3FH

Scale

Elevation

Amplitude

R2

Angular displacement

CI accuracy

Dependent Obsessive-compulsive Affective lability Anxiousness Depressiveness Fantasy Health anxiety Irresponsibility Non-perseverance Peculiarity Perfectionism Self-harm Workaholism Na: Anxiousness Na: Depressivity Dis: Distractibility Psy: Eccentricity Na: Emotional lability Dis: Irresponsibility Na: Perseveration Na: Separation insecurity Disinhibition Negative affect N: Impulsiveness N: Vulnerability C: -Competence C: -Self-discipline O: -Ideas O: -Fantasy Neuroticism Conscientiousness

.31 (.24, .37) .16 (.10, .23) .44 (.35, .51) .37 (.27, .47) .34 (.24, .44) .38 (.27, .48) .35 (.24, .44) .16 (.06, .28) .40 (.30, .48) .41 (.31, .50) .08 (⫺.05, .21) .23 (.10, .35) .11 (0, .21) .43 (.38, .47) .46 (.41, .51) .43 (.37, .48) .38 (.32, .44) .41 (.36, .46) .35 (.28, .42) .43 (.37, .38) .33 (.27, .39) .43 (.37, .49) .45 (.40, .49) .23 (.17, .30) .36 (.31, .42) .33 (.27, .40) .29 (.23, .35) .11 (.04, .19) .05 (⫺.01, .11) .40 (.35, .45) .31 (.25, .37)

.02 (.01, .08) .09 (.04, .14) .08 (.02, .16) .06 (.02, .15) .09 (.02, .20) .04 (.01, .14) .05 (.01, .14) .02 (.01, .14) .05 (.01, .15) .03 (.01, .13) .07 (.02, .19) .02 (.01, .11) .02 (.01, .16) .05 (.02, .10) .09 (.05, .13) .05 (.02, .11) .08 (.03, .14) .03 (.01, .09) .09 (.04, .14) .02 (.01, .08) .06 (.02, .12) .07 (.03, .12) .01 (.01, .07) .08 (.03, .14) .04 (.01, .10) .03 (.01, .09) .09 (.04, .15) .05 (.01, .12) .10 (.04, .16) .04 (.01, .08) .04 (.01, .08)

.28 .72 .69 .55 .72 .61 .41 .03 .48 .27 .51 .11 .20 .48 .77 .92 .83 .49 .76 .47 .73 .76 .13 .71 .69 .78 .78 .61 .59 .53 .31

294.6 (139.7, 99.6) 115.6 (119.9, 195.3) 151.3 (97.3, 219.9) 265.1 (150, 347) 180.4 (127.8, 270.3) 108.7 (316.1, 242.3) 125.4 (337.7, 276) 142.5 (329.8, 303.4) 213.6 (91.8, 347.3) 104.1 (297, 255.8) 108.4 (349.9, 210.1) 112.1 (308.5, 285.1) 126.4 (313.5, 287.9) 242.7 (174.2, 302.8) 227.6 (191.3, 262.1) 281.5 (206.1, 331.4) 127.1 (84.6, 165.2) 21.4 (285.7, 139.8) 120.8 (88, 152.6) 295.5 (148.3, 80.8) 78.7 (18.3, 124.7) 114.3 (66, 151.4) 56.1 (278.1, 210) 49 (359.4, 93.9) 285.8 (171.2, 38.4) 254.3 (142.1, 354.6) 299.2 (249.8, 332.1) 154.7 (71.2, 239.1) 190.6 (152.3, 231.6) 242.9 (137.2, 319.5) 320.9 (182.3, 85.2)

.11 .89 .32 .25 .53 .12 .11 .10 .17 .08 .26 .05 .11 .43 .74 .57 .75 .27 .87 .13 .75 .75 .06 .59 .30 1.00 .85 .47 .92 .24 .21

Note. All scales had amplitudes ⬍ .10 and thus angular displacement values should be interpreted with caution. Personality trait domains are shown in bold. Confidence intervals should only be interpreted when the probability of their accuracy is ⬎ .50.

els differ in their ability to provide such interpersonally relevant information about individuals. When choosing a PD measure or explaining clinical phenomena, clinicians should consider the extent to which PD models provide diverse ways to account for these problems. It is then comforting that the PID-5 and CAT-PD facets converged in their relations with interpersonal problems. These models were constructed from the bottom-up by independent teams (Simms et al., 2011; Krueger et al., 2012); thus this agreement lends confidence to the mapping of lower-order PD traits. Despite this, some CAT-PD traits may present unique contributions to PD diagnosis. For instance, CAT-PD Emotional Detachment provides an additional way to describe individuals with Socially Avoidant problems and the traits Norm Violation and Rudeness do so similarly for Domineering problems. Beyond this, neither model covered warm or purely submissive problems very well. Future work may consider additional constructs (e.g., Self-Effacing; Gore, Presnall, Miller, Lynam, & Widiger, 2012) to expand their interpersonal coverage.

Limitations and Future Directions The present study benefitted from a large patient sample, broad representation of PD models, and a nuanced interpersonal model. An additional strength was the disaggregation of dysfunction se-

verity from style. Despite these strengths, the present results should be evaluated in the context their limitations. First, generalizability may be limited to White and African American populations, as other ethnic groups largely were absent from our sample; however, personality-IIP relations generally have replicated across cultures in previous studies (Monsen et al., 2006; Simonsen & Simonsen, 2009). Additionally, generalizability may be slightly impacted given that male, African American participants with slightly more severe personality pathology were less likely to have completed all questions and thus were excluded. Although it is possible that that these excluded individuals would show different effects, we know of no research supporting such a concern. Second, all data were self-reported, whereas previous research has indicated the value of informant reports (e.g., Clifton, Turkheimer, & Oltmanns, 2005). In particular, some of the traits and problems investigated are highly “evaluative,” or undesirable, meaning that individuals may struggle to admit to or lack knowledge of these characteristics (e.g., Carlson, Vazire, & Oltmanns, 2013). Future work in this domain would benefit from examining the interpersonal impact of these PD models from the perspective of individuals who know the patients well. There also were several measure-related limitations. In the present study, ASPD could not be examined in full, although conduct disorder symptoms related to interpersonal problems in a manner

WILLIAMS AND SIMMS

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

26

similar to previous findings with ASPD (e.g., Pincus & Wiggins, 1990). In addition, we used the NEO-PI-3FH to assess FFM facets. Some of these scales have poor internal consistency, potentially attenuating correlations and explaining the lower elevation parameters for NEO facets. Full NEO-PI-3 facet scales have better internal consistency; however, even the full NEO scales have generally lower alphas than PID-5 and CAT-PD-SF scales (McCrae, Costa, & Martin, 2005). An additional concern is that the NEO may be an inadequate representation of the FFM of PD (e.g., Widiger, 2011). Recently developed PD-specific faceted FFM measures assess maladaptive variants of FFM traits (e.g., Gore et al., 2012) and may provide broader coverage of personality pathology than the NEO; however, further work is needed to integrate the scales from these individual models into a broader descriptive model. For instance, a number of these measures contain pathological versions of the same facet, but with different names (Lynam, 2012). Future research is needed to integrate across these new measures and demonstrate their relations to interpersonal problems.

Conclusion Past findings relating DSM–5 PD models to interpersonal problems were replicated and competing PD trait models converged in relations to interpersonal problems at the domain-level, as previous research would predict (e.g., Wright & Simms, 2014). Beyond this, the finding that pathological and normal-range trait models differ in their ability to describe individuals with domineering versus nonassertive problems at the facet-level adds to our understanding of the lower-order structure of PD trait models. Importantly, these facet-level findings suggest that these models may have nontrivial differences in the constructs they include and, more generally, point to the importance of comparing models at the facet-level. Finally, across all models, few traits or PDs related to warm interpersonal problems, suggesting they are underemphasized or less central to common PD features. Future work is needed to replicate the present findings and identify a comprehensive set of PD traits that reflect the full range of interpersonal problems seen in clinical practice.

References Alden, L. E., Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1990). Construction of circumplex scales for the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 521–536. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 00223891.1990.9674088 American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Antony, M. M., & Roemer, L. (2003). Behavior therapy. In A. S. Gurman & S. B. Messer (Eds.), Essential psychotherapies: Theory and practice (2nd ed., pp. 182–223). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117 .3.497 Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187–215. http://dx.doi .org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.187 Carlson, E. N., Vazire, S., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2013). Self-other knowledge asymmetries in personality pathology. Journal of Personality, 81, 155– 170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00794.x

Clark, L. A. (2007). Assessment and diagnosis of personality disorder: Perennial issues and an emerging reconceptualization. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 227–257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57 .102904.190200 Clifton, A., Turkheimer, E., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2005). Self- and peer perspectives on pathological personality traits and interpersonal problems. Psychological Assessment, 17, 123–131. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/1040-3590.17.2.123 Coker, L. A., Samuel, D. B., & Widiger, T. A. (2002). Maladaptive personality functioning within the big five and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality Disorders, 16, 385– 401. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1521/pedi.16.5.385.22125 Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO–PI–R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO–FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. W. (1995). The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–III–R Personality Disorders (SCID-II): I. Description. Journal of Personality Disorders, 9, 83–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.1995.9.2.83 Fleeson, W., & Jayawickreme, E. (2015). Whole trait theory. Journal of Research in Personality, 56, 82–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014 .10.009 Gore, W. L., & Pincus, A. L. (2013). Dependency and the five factor model. In T. A. Widiger & P. T. Costa (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (3rd ed., pp. 163–177). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 13939-011 Gore, W. L., Presnall, J. R., Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2012). A five-factor measure of dependent personality traits. Journal of Personality Assessment, 94, 488 – 499. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 00223891.2012.670681 Gurtman, M. B. (1992). Construct validity of interpersonal personality measures: The interpersonal circumplex as a nomological net. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 105–118. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0022-3514.63.1.105 Hopwood, C. J., Ansell, E. B., Pincus, A. L., Wright, A. G. C., Lukowitsky, M. R., & Roche, M. J. (2011). The circumplex structure of interpersonal sensitivities. Journal of Personality, 79, 707–740. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00696.x Hopwood, C. J., Burt, S. A., Keel, P. K., Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., & Klump, K. L. (2013). Interpersonal problems associated with multidimensional personality questionnaire traits in women during the transition to adulthood. Assessment, 20, 60 – 67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 1073191111425854 Hopwood, C. J., Koonce, E. A., & Morey, L. C. (2009). An exploratory study of integrative personality pathology systems and the interpersonal circumplex. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31, 331–339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10862-008-9124-y Hopwood, C. J., Wright, A. G. C., Ansell, E. B., & Pincus, A. L. (2013). The interpersonal core of personality pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27, 270 –295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2013.27.3.270 Kendler, K. S. (2009). An historical framework for psychiatric nosology. Psychological Medicine, 39, 1935–1941. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ S0033291709005753 Krueger, R. F., Derringer, J., Markon, K. E., Watson, D., & Skodol, A. E. (2012). Initial construction of a maladaptive personality trait model and inventory for DSM–5. Psychological Medicine, 42, 1879 –1890. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711002674 Krueger, R. F., Eaton, N. R., Clark, L. A., Watson, D., Markon, K. E., Derringer, J., . . . Livesley, W. J. (2011). Deriving an empirical structure of personality pathology for DSM–5. Journal of Personality Disorders, 25, 170 –191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2011.25.2.170 Leary, T. (1957). Interpersonal diagnosis of personality. New York, NY: Ronald Press.

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

PERSONALITY DISORDER AND INTERPERSONAL DYSFUNCTION Locke, K. D., & Sadler, P. (2007). Self-efficacy, values, and complementarity in dyadic interactions: Integrating interpersonal and socialcognitive theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 94 – 109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167206293375 Lukowitsky, M. R., & Pincus, A. L. (2011). The pantheoretical nature of mental representations and their ability to predict interpersonal adjustment in a nonclinical sample. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 28, 48 –74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020849 Lynam, D. R. (2012). Assessment of maladaptive variants of Five-Factor Model traits. Journal of Personality, 80, 1593–1613. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00775.x McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1989). The structure of interpersonal traits: Wiggins’s circumplex and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 586 –595. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0022-3514.56.4.586 McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2007). Brief versions of the NEO-PI-3. Journal of Individual Differences, 28, 116 –128. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1027/1614-0001.28.3.116 McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., & Martin, T. A. (2005). The NEO-PI-3: A more readable revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 84, 261–270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/ s15327752jpa8403_05 Monsen, J. T., Hagtvet, K. A., Havik, O. E., & Eilertsen, D. E. (2006). Circumplex structure and personality disorder correlates of the Interpersonal Problems model (IIP-C): Construct validity and clinical implications. Psychological Assessment, 18, 165–173. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.165 Pincus, A. L., Lukowitsky, M. R., & Wright, A. G. C. (2010). The interpersonal nexus of personality and psychopathology. In T. Millon, R. F. Krueger, & E. Simonsen (Eds.), Contemporary directions in psychopathology: Scientific foundations for DSM–5 and ICD-11 (pp. 523–552). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Pincus, A. L., & Wiggins, J. S. (1990). Interpersonal problems and conceptions of personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 4, 342–352. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.1990.4.4.342 Roche, M. J., Pincus, A. L., Hyde, A. L., Conroy, D. E., & Ram, N. (2013). Within-person covariation of agentic and communal perceptions: Implications for interpersonal theory and assessment. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 445– 452. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.01.007 Ryder, A. G., Costa, P. T., Jr., & Bagby, R. M. (2007). Evaluation of the SCID-II personality disorder traits for DSM–IV: Coherence, discrimination, relations with general personality traits, and functional impairment. Journal of Personality Disorders, 21, 626 – 637. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1521/pedi.2007.21.6.626 Safran, J. D. (1990). Towards a refinement of cognitive therapy in light of interpersonal theory: II. Practice. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 107– 121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(90)90109-N Samuel, D. B. (2011). Assessing personality in the DSM–5: The utility of bipolar constructs. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 390 –397. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2011.577476 Simms, L. J., Goldberg, L. R., Roberts, J. E., Watson, D., Welte, J., & Rotterman, J. H. (2011). Computerized adaptive assessment of personality disorder: Introducing the CAT-PD project. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 380 –389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2011 .577475 Simonsen, S., & Simonsen, E. (2009). The Danish DAPP-BQ: Reliability, factor structure, and convergence with SCID-II and IIP-C. Journal of

27

Personality Disorders, 23, 629 – 646. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi .2009.23.6.629 Skodol, A. E. (2012). Personality disorders in DSM–5. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 317–344. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurevclinpsy-032511-143131 Soldz, S., Budman, S., Demby, A., & Merry, J. (1993). Representation of personality disorders in circumplex and five-factor space: Explorations with a clinical sample. Psychological Assessment, 5, 41–52. http://dx .doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.5.1.41 Soldz, S., Budman, S., Demby, A., & Merry, J. (1995). A short form of the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems Circumplex Scales. Assessment, 2, 53– 63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191195002001006 Ullrich, S., Deasy, D., Smith, J., Johnson, B., Clarke, M., Broughton, N., & Coid, J. (2008). Detecting personality disorders in the prison population of England and Wales: Comparing case identification using the SCID-II screen and the SCID-II clinical interview. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 19, 301–322. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 14789940802045182 Watson, D., Stasik, S. M., Ro, E., & Clark, L. A. (2013). Integrating normal and pathological personality: Relating the DSM–5 traitdimensional model to general traits of personality. Assessment, 20, 312–326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191113485810 Widiger, T. A. (2011). Integrating normal and abnormal personality structure: A proposal for DSM-V. Journal of Personality Disorders, 25, 338 –363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2011.25.3.338 Widiger, T. A., & Samuel, D. B. (2005). Diagnostic categories or dimensions? A question for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–fifth ed. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 494 –504. Williams, T. F., Thomas, K. M., Donnellan, M. B., & Hopwood, C. J. (2014). The aversive interpersonal behaviors associated with pathological personality traits. Journal of Personality Disorders, 28, 824 – 840. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2014_28_139 Wright, A. G. C., Hallquist, M. N., Beeney, J. E., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2013). Borderline personality pathology and the stability of interpersonal problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 1094 –1100. http://dx.doi .org/10.1037/a0034658 Wright, A. G. C., Hallquist, M. N., Morse, J. Q., Scott, L. N., Stepp, S. D., Nolf, K. A., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2013). Clarifying interpersonal heterogeneity in borderline personality disorder using latent mixture modeling. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27, 125–143. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1521/pedi.2013.27.2.125 Wright, A. G. C., Pincus, A. L., Conroy, D. E., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2009). Integrating methods to optimize circumplex description and comparison of groups. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91, 311–322. http://dx.doi .org/10.1080/00223890902935696 Wright, A. G. C., Pincus, A. L., Hopwood, C. J., Thomas, K. M., Markon, K. E., & Krueger, R. F. (2012). An interpersonal analysis of pathological personality traits in DSM–5. Assessment, 19, 263–275. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1177/1073191112446657 Wright, A. G. C., & Simms, L. J. (2014). On the structure of personality disorder traits: Conjoint analyses of the CAT-PD, PID-5, and NEO-PI-3 trait models. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 5, 43–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/per0000037 Zimmerman, J. & Wright, A. G. C. (2015). Beyond description in interpersonal construct validation: Methodological advances in the circumplex structural summary approach. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Personality disorder models and their coverage of interpersonal problems.

Interpersonal dysfunction is a defining feature of personality disorders (PDs) and can serve as a criterion for comparing PD models. In this study, th...
199KB Sizes 2 Downloads 6 Views