Research Report

Psychological reactivity to laboratory stress is associated with hormonal responses in postmenopausal women

Journal of International Medical Research 2014, Vol. 42(2) 444–456 ! The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0300060513504696 imr.sagepub.com

Carolyn Y Fang1, Brian L Egleston2, Angelica M Manzur3, Raymond R Townsend4, Frank Z Stanczyk5, David Spiegel6 and Joanne F Dorgan7

Abstract Objective: The present study examined associations between psychological reactivity and hormonal responses to a standardized laboratory stressor (Trier Social Stress Test, TSST) in postmenopausal women. Methods: Postmenopausal women aged 50–74 years undertook anxiety and mood assessments prior to and following the TSST. Blood samples were drawn at multiple timepoints for assessment of cortisol, adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) and dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA). Results: Forty postmenopausal women completed the assessments. As expected, significant increases in anxiety and negative affect and decreases in positive affect were observed after the TSST; however, the magnitude of change in anxiety and mood varied considerably across individuals. Analyses indicated that greater increases in anxiety and negative affect after the TSST were associated with higher levels of cortisol, ACTH and DHEA after controlling for race, age, body mass index and smoking status. Changes in positive affect were not associated with cortisol, ACTH or DHEA. Conclusions: These findings suggest that enhanced reactivity to stress is associated with higher hormone levels among postmenopausal women, which could have potential implications for health.

1

Cancer Prevention and Control Program, Fox Chase Cancer Center, Philadelphia, PA, USA 2 Department of Biostatistics and Bioinformatics, Fox Chase Cancer Center, Philadelphia, PA, USA 3 Northwestern Memorial Hospital, Chicago, IL, USA 4 Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA 5 Reproductive Endocrine Research Laboratory, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA

6

School of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA 7 Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University of Maryland, Baltimore, Baltimore, MD, USA Corresponding author: Carolyn Y Fang, Cancer Prevention and Control Program, Fox Chase Cancer Center, Robert C. Young Pavilion, 4th Floor, 333 Cottman Avenue, Philadelphia, PA 19111, USA. Email: [email protected]

Creative Commons CC-BY-NC: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 3.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original workOF is BIRMINGHAM attributed as on specified on the SAGE and Open Access page Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV June 4, 2015 (http://www.uk.sagepub.com/aboutus/openaccess.htm).

Fang et al.

445

Keywords Trier Social Stress Test, anxiety, negative affect, cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone, adrenocorticotrophic hormone Date received: 22 May 2013; accepted: 16 June 2013

Introduction The neuroendocrine stress response has been extensively studied using both naturalistic and laboratory stress paradigms.1 As a result, it is well established that stress exposure leads to corticotrophin-releasing hormone (CRH) and arginine vasopressin (AVP) discharge from the hypothalamus.2 CRH and AVP synergize to stimulate pituitary adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) production, which increases adrenal production of cortisol and various androgens, including dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and its sulphate (DHEAS).3 Stress-related alterations in hypothalamic–pituitary– adrenal (HPA) axis function and its effects on ACTH, cortisol and DHEA have been described in many studies and across various populations.4 The effects of stress on DHEA and DHEAS may be particularly relevant for breast cancer risk in postmenopausal women. Several prospective studies have reported that elevated levels of these adrenal androgens are associated with increased risk of breast cancer among postmenopausal women.5–9 In a pooled analysis of prospective studies,10 postmenopausal women with elevated DHEA and DHEAS, which are almost exclusively derived from the adrenals,11 were twice as likely to develop breast cancer compared with women with low levels. The cortisol level has also been reported to be elevated in women with breast cancer12 and may increase risk indirectly through its effects on metabolism: for example, cortisol increases adiposity,13 an established risk factor for breast cancer in postmenopausal women.14

Despite the well-documented effects of stress on HPA axis function and its downstream hormonal pathways, studies of stress and breast cancer risk have yielded mixed results.15 Some studies have noted a positive association between stress and breast cancer risk,16–18 whereas others have not.19–22 The mixed findings may be due to a variety of factors, including behavioral, biological and reproductive factors, and differences in psychological reactivity to stress.15 Studies have noted that variations in psychological stress reactivity lead to differential physiological responses to stress.23 Indeed, measures of sympathetic nervous system activity (e.g. cardiac activation and systemic vascular resistance) have been found to vary by participants’ perceptions of the stressor.24,25 The Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) is a standardized protocol consisting of evaluative speech and mental arithmetic tasks, which has been commonly used to induce social stress in a laboratory setting. The TSST has been found to reliably stimulate ACTH, cortisol and DHEA in healthy men and women,4,26,27 although considerable variation in the magnitude of response exists.28 Variation in neuroendocrine reactivity to the TSST has been investigated with respect to a variety of factors, including race,29 sex,30 age31 and various clinical conditions,32–34 but data also suggest that subjective psychological responses (e.g. affect, mood, perceived stress) are associated with cortisol reactivity.28,35 For example, increases in stress and negative affect were positively associated with increased cortisol reactivity in studies of adult men36,37 and adolescents.38 Studies have also reported stress-induced increases in DHEA

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

446

Journal of International Medical Research 42(2)

during the TSST, including large variations in the magnitude of DHEA response.26,27 Thus, the purpose of the present study was to examine differences in psychological reactivity to a standardized laboratory stressor (TSST) in relation to hormonal reactivity among postmenopausal women. Specifically, it was hypothesized that greater increases in anxiety and negative affect, and decreases in positive affect, after the TSST would be associated with greater ACTH, cortisol and DHEA reactivity to the TSST.

Subjects and methods Criteria for participation The study included healthy postmenopausal women aged 50–74 years. The exclusion criteria were: having both ovaries removed; history of cancer other than nonmelanoma skin cancer; history of Cushing’s or Addison’s disease, coronary heart disease, stroke, uncontrolled hypertension or other health conditions that could place the participant at risk; Alzheimer’s disease or cognitive deficit; taking any medications that could interfere with assessment of biological outcomes such as oestrogens, progestogens, androgens, prednisone or psychoactive drugs.

Recruitment Participants were recruited between February 2008 and June 2009, using local newspapers from the Philadelphia, Pennsylvania region, and online classified advertisements. Eligible women who consented to participate provided written informed consent and were scheduled for the TSST.

Ethics This study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Fox Chase Cancer Center

(IRB no. 07-840) and the University of Pennsylvania (IRB no. 808042).

Administration of the TSST and blood sampling Depending on participant location, the TSST procedures were conducted at either the Fox Chase Cancer Center Clinical Research Unit or the University of Pennsylvania Clinical and Translational Research Center. The TSST was administered during a 3-h visit using a modification of the protocol by Singh et al.39,40 Participants were instructed to abstain from caffeine, alcohol, cigarettes and strenuous activities for 24 h and to fast for between 2 h and 3 h before arriving for the TSST. Participants were also instructed to arrive 75 min before the start of the TSST protocol, which began at 13:00 h for all participants. First, participants’ characteristics and mood were recorded. At 60 min before the test, participants drank water (5 ml/kg) to insure uniform hydration, and 40 min before the test, an intravenous (i.v.) catheter (with saline lock) was placed in the forearm vein for blood sampling. The TSST was immediately preceded by a 20-min waiting period, during which participants were instructed to sit quietly, then the TSST was administered. Specific tasks in the TSST included preparing for (5 min) and then delivering (5 min) a speech and a mental arithmetic task (5 min). Following the TSST, participants sat quietly during a recovery period. Blood samples for ACTH, cortisol and DHEA were collected through the i.v. catheter at 1 min before (1 min) and 1, 15 and 30 min after the TSST, and additionally for cortisol and DHEA at 60 and 90 min following the TSST. Immediately following the last blood sampling point, participants completed measures of post-TSST anxiety and mood. Blood samples were collected using one 5-ml purple-top (ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid) plastic tube and one 10-ml

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

Fang et al.

447

red-top (plain) glass tube and held at room temperature for 30 min to allow complete clotting. After aliquoting, samples were stored at 80 C. All blood samples were transported to Fox Chase Cancer Center for processing and storage (for 18 months) prior to analysis.

indicating more positive affect. Similarly, the 10 negative affect items were summed to create a negative affect score, with higher levels indicating more negative affect and distress. In the present sample, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.86 for Positive Affect and 0.83 for Negative Affect.

Participant characteristics

Hormone assays

The height, weight and waist circumference of each participant were measured. For demographic and socio-economic characteristics, participants completed a brief questionnaire that contained items on demographic background, health behavior, age, education, race/ethnicity, marital status and smoking.

Cortisol, ACTH and DHEA assays were conducted in the Reproductive Endocrine Research Laboratory at the Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California. All samples were assayed in duplicate. Blood samples were batched at Fox Chase Cancer Center and shipped overnight on dry ice to the laboratory. Each participant’s samples were assayed together in the same batch. Cortisol was measured by a solid-phase competitive chemiluminescent enzyme immunoassay on an ImmuliteÕ 2000 analyser (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany).47 The interassay coefficient of variation (CV) was 7.3%. The sensitivity of the assay is reported to be 20 mg/dl. Plasma samples for ACTH were collected on ice and processed within 1 h of collection. ACTH was measured by a solid-phase twosite sequential chemiluminescent immunometric assay on an ImmuliteÕ 2000 analyser.48 The interassay CV was 8.7%. The sensitivity of the assay is reported to be 5 pg/ml. Next, DHEA in serum was extracted and measured by radioimmunoassay.6 Briefly, following extraction using ethyl acetate/ hexane (2 : 3) and evaporation of the organic solvents,5 the residue was redissolved in isooctane and applied on a Celite column impregnated with ethylene glycol. DHEA was eluted using 15% toluene in iso-octane. After evaporation of the eluate, the residue was reconstituted in assay buffer and aliquots were taken for radioimmunoassay. The radioimmunoassay utilized a highly

Anxiety and mood scales Levels of anxiety are commonly assessed prior to and following the administration of the TSST.41 Self-reported anxiety was measured using the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), which is a 21-item list of anxietyrelated symptoms and feelings.42,43 Items were summed to obtain a total score, with higher scores reflecting greater anxiety. Cronbach’s a in the present sample was 0.90 indicating high internal consistency. Mood was assessed using the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS).44 This 20-item scale comprises two mood scales: one assessing positive affect and the other measuring negative affect. This well-validated scale has been widely used in previous studies of stress reactivity, in which increases in negative affect and decreases in positive affect were observed following the TSST.32,45,46 Each of the 20 items is rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 ¼ ‘Very slightly or not at all’ to 5 ¼ ‘Extremely’ and participants are instructed to indicate the extent to which they felt at the time. The 10 positive affect items were summed to create a positive affect score, with higher levels

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

448

Journal of International Medical Research 42(2)

specific antiserum in conjunction with an iodinated radioligand. Following incubation, antibody-bound and unbound DHEA was separated by incubation with a second antibody. After centrifugation for 15–20 min at 1500 g at room temperature, antibody-bound DHEA was then quantified. The interassay CV was 10.5%. The sensitivity of the assay is reported as 0.2 ng/ml.

Statistical analyses Preliminary analyses were conducted to examine potential socio-economic and behavioral covariates of baseline neuroendocrine measures using Pearson’s correlation coefficient and one-way analysis of variance. Next, to obtain a summary measure for each neuroendocrine marker, the area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (AUC) was computed using the trapezoid formula.49 Peak reactivity ( peak) was computed using the difference score between the maximum level of each neuroendocrine marker and the pre-TSST baseline value.31 Changes in anxiety and mood ratings for each participant were computed by subtracting the pre-TSST rating from the post-TSST rating, with higher change scores reflecting greater increases in anxiety, negative affect and positive affect. Correlational analyses were then performed to examine associations among changes in anxiety and mood. Because changes in anxiety and negative affect were likely to be highly correlated with each other, a composite variable of psychological reactivity was created by computing z-scores for change in anxiety and negative affect.50 The z-scores were then combined into a composite score, where higher values reflected greater increases in psychological reactivity. This composite measure was examined in relation to neuroendocrine reactivity using multivariable regression models. To control for potential

confounding socio-economic or behavioral variables that were observed to be associated (P  0.10) with neuroendocrine response in preliminary analyses, these variables were entered as covariates in regression models. Post-hoc analyses were also conducted to explore whether individual characteristics, such as smoking status and body mass index (BMI), were associated with baseline levels of cortisol, ACTH or DHEA. For BMI post-hoc analyses, we divided participants using a median split into two groups: (1) those with BMI 26 kg/m2; (2) those with BMI >26 kg/m2. Multivariable regression models were used to explore whether smoking status and BMI were associated with reactivity. All analyses controlled for baseline levels of the neuroendocrine marker. Analyses were conducted using SPSSÕ Statistics version 21.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Results A total of 40 women agreed to participate in this study. The characteristics of the participants are presented in Table 1. In preliminary analyses, BMI was positively correlated with higher baseline levels of ACTH (r ¼ 0.32, P < 0.05). Post-hoc analyses revealed that women with BMI >26 kg/m2 had significantly greater ACTH reactivity, as measured by AUC and peak reactivity, compared with women with BMI 26 kg/m2. There was no association between BMI and cortisol or DHEA reactivity. Also in preliminary analyses, never smokers had higher baseline cortisol levels (mean  standard deviation, 7.52  3.26 mg/ dl) than former smokers (5.83  2.40 mg/dl) and current smokers (4.80  2.76 mg/dl; F(2,39) ¼ 2.97, P ¼ 0.06). Caucasians had higher baseline cortisol levels (7.38  3.06 mg/dl) than non-Caucasians (5.42  2.81 mg/dl; F(1,39) ¼ 4.40, P ¼ 0.05). Age was negatively associated with baseline DHEA levels (r ¼ 0.34, P < 0.05).

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

Fang et al.

449

Table 1. Characteristics of participants recruited for a study of associations between psychological reactivity and hormonal responses to a standardized laboratory stressor in postmenopausal women. Study population, n ¼ 40

Characteristic Age, years Race Caucasian African American Asian American/Pacific Islander American Indian/Alaska Native Body mass index, kg/m2 Marital status Single (never married) Married/living as married Separated/divorced/widowed Education High school or less Some college College or postgraduate degree Smoking history Never Former Current

56.3  4.69 50.0 42.5 5.0 2.5 27.6  5.94 20.0 42.5 37.5 17.5 55.0 27.5 47.5 30.0 22.5

Data presented as mean  SD or %.

Therefore, BMI, race, smoking status and age were included as covariates in subsequent analyses. As expected, anxiety and negative affect significantly increased from pre- to postTSST, whereas positive affect significantly decreased (see Table 2). However, the extent of change was quite variable across participants. For example, with respect to anxiety, change scores ranged from –7 to 37, indicating that some participants reported a slight decrease in anxiety (as reflected by a negative change score) from pre- to post-TSST. After controlling for BMI, race, smoking status and age, the correlation analyses indicated that changes in anxiety and negative affect were significantly associated with cortisol, ACTH and DHEA reactivity

(Table 3). Specifically, increases in anxiety and negative affect were positively correlated with cortisol AUC and peak reactivity, and ACTH AUC and peak reactivity. Increase in anxiety was positively associated with DHEA AUC and peak reactivity. Negative affect was positively associated with DHEA peak reactivity but not with DHEA AUC (P ¼ 0.056). Changes in anxiety and negative affect were significantly correlated with each other (r ¼ 0.55, P < 0.001), but neither change in anxiety nor negative affect was associated with change in positive affect (r ¼ 0.06 and 0.10, both P > 0.57). Changes in positive affect were not correlated with neuroendocrine reactivity, therefore positive affect was excluded from subsequent regression analyses. Using standardized scores, a composite measure of psychological reactivity was computed. Multiple linear regression analyses were then performed to examine associations between psychological reactivity and neuroendocrine response. With respect to cortisol, regression analyses indicated that larger increases in psychological reactivity were significantly associated with greater cortisol reactivity, as measured by AUC ( ¼ 0.55, P ¼ 0.001) and peak reactivity ( ¼ 0.52, P ¼ 0.002), controlling for relevant covariates (i.e. BMI, race, age, smoking status and baseline cortisol levels). Similarly, greater increases in psychological reactivity were significantly associated with greater ACTH reactivity, as measured by AUC ( ¼ 0.50, P ¼ 0.004) and peak reactivity ( ¼ 0.53, P ¼ 0.002), controlling for relevant covariates. Finally, greater increases in psychological reactivity were significantly associated with greater changes in DHEA peak reactivity ( ¼ 0.33, P < 0.04), whereas the association between psychological reactivity and DHEA AUC did not reach statistical significance. To illustrate such associations, participants were categorized as high or low

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

450

Journal of International Medical Research 42(2)

Table 2. Overall self-reported anxiety and mood before and after administration of the Trier Social Stress Test in a study of associations between psychological reactivity and hormonal responses to a standardized laboratory stressor in postmenopausal women. Parameter

Before

After

t-value

Change

Anxietya Negative affectb Positive affectb

3.33  5.96 13.04  5.53 32.53  8.26

11.02  10.21 16.97  6.65 28.14  8.21

4.60*** 4.15*** 4.24***

7.68  10.43 (7 to 37) 3.93  5.99 (6 to 23) 4.53  6.67 (18 to 7)

Data presented as mean  SD or mean  SD (range). ***P < 0.001 (Student’s t-test). a BAI, Beck Anxiety Inventory, maximum score ¼ 63. b PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Scale, maximum score ¼ 10.

Table 3. Correlations between psychological and hormonal responses in a study of association between psychological reactivity and hormonal responses to a standardized laboratory stressor in postmenopausal women. Cortisol

ACTH

DHEA

Change

AUC

Peak reactivity

AUC

Peak reactivity

AUC

Peak reactivity

Anxietya Positive affectb Negative affectb

0.43** 0.22 0.52**

0.36* 0.19 0.55**

0.50** 0.14 0.41*

0.52** 0.14 0.43**

0.35* 0.13 0.32

0.37* 0.15 0.36*

*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01 (Pearson’s correlation coefficient). a Beck Anxiety Inventory. b Positive and Negative Affect Scale. ACTH, adrenocorticotrophic hormone; AUC, area under the curve; DHEA, dehydroepiandrosterone.

psychological responders, based on a median split of their composite psychological reactivity change scores.38 Levels of cortisol, ACTH and DHEA were then plotted according to psychological response status (Figure 1). For high responders, a more pronounced peak was observed for each neuroendocrine marker compared with low responders. Further, among high responders, the peak level of ACTH was reached at 1 min post TSST, with levels declining rapidly thereafter. In contrast, peak levels of cortisol and DHEA were highest at 1–15 min post TSST.

Discussion The present study findings suggest that greater psychological reactivity to a laboratory stress is associated with greater hormonal responses among postmenopausal women. Specifically, women who reported larger increases in anxiety and negative affect following TSST exposure had significantly higher levels of cortisol, ACTH and DHEA. These findings are consistent with prior research that reported that stress-induced increases in negative affect are associated with neuroendocrine reactivity.36

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

Fang et al.

451

(a) 16 Cortisol (µg/dl)

14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0

///// –1

1

15

30

60

90

1

15

30

60

90

1

15

30

60

90

(b) 35 ACTH (pg/ml)

30 25 20 15 10 5 0

///// –1

(c) 16 DHEA (µg/ml)

14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 –1

///// TSST

Elapsed time (min)

Figure 1. (a) Cortisol, (b) ACTH and (c) DHEA levels in postmenopausal women according to psychological response status (low and high responder according to a median split of composite psychological reactivity change scores; both n ¼ 20) at baseline (1 min), immediately after the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST; 1 min), and during the recovery period (15–90 min). ACTH was measured only up to 30 min. Diamonds, low responders (score 0.48); squares, high responders (score >0.48). ACTH, adrenocorticotrophic hormone; DHEA, dehydroepiandrosterone. The color version of this figure is available at: http://imr.sagepub.com.

Change in positive affect, however, is not significantly associated with neuroendocrine reactivity. Others have also noted no association between positive affect and neuroendocrine responses to the TSST,31,45,46 although one study of adult women reported

that greater decreases in positive outlook (operationalized as positive affect and cognitions) after the TSST were associated with greater increases in proinflammatory interleukin-1 reactivity to the TSST.51 In observational studies, positive affect is widely

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

452

Journal of International Medical Research 42(2)

reported to have health-protective effects.52,53 Positive affect is generally associated with lower diurnal cortisol levels54–57 and a smaller rise in the cortisol awakening response.58–60 Cortisol is a marker of adrenocortical activity that is associated with psychosocial and physical stress.61 It may be that sustained positive affect is needed in order to observe any beneficial and protective effects of positive affect on neuroendocrine reactivity, with the mood-induction paradigms and stress tasks employed in the laboratory being too brief to engender any consistent and reliable association between positive affect and physiological outcomes. Other characteristics, such as smoking status and BMI, were associated with baseline levels of cortisol and ACTH, respectively. However, post-hoc analyses indicated no differences in cortisol, ACTH and DHEA reactivity between smokers and nonsmokers. The observed relationship between BMI and ACTH is consistent with prior studies demonstrating that obese women show altered responsiveness of the HPA axis.62 Specifically, data indicate that obesity is associated with alterations in secretary patterns of ACTH, but not cortisol, throughout the day.63 Other studies have also noted that cortisol responses to the TSST do not differ between obese and nonobese women.64 Rather, it has been proposed that abnormalities in HPA axis reactivity are more likely to be observed by specific phenotypes of body fat distribution (i.e. visceral versus peripheral),63 which may be more informative for characterizing the health effects of obesity than BMI alone. Further, the results suggest that individual differences in psychological reactivity to the TSST may be an important factor to consider when investigating potential differences in HPA axis response between groups. Indeed, the considerable variability in emotional reactivity was manifested by

differential hormonal responses: specifically, greater increases in ACTH, cortisol and DHEA in women who were high responders compared with low responders. Given that women do not respond to the same stressor in identical ways, it is not surprising that studies of stress and breast cancer have yielded such diverse results. For example, in a matched case–control study, higher risk of breast cancer was not associated with a history of childhood adversity (i.e. stress condition) per se, but rather with that, coupled with reports of family support.65 As a result, differences in stress reactivity have implications for how future studies of stress and breast cancer risk are carried out and interpreted. The present study has several limitations. First, the sample size was relatively small. However, given that the study was focused on postmenopausal women, heterogeneity in biologically relevant factors that may be present in other larger studies (such as gender and menstrual cycle timing) was minimized. Second, although the study sample was diverse with respect to racial background, it was homogeneous for age and menopausal status and, therefore, the findings cannot be generalized to younger women. Third, to reduce participant burden during data collection, psychological stress responses were not assessed during the TSST. Because the psychological stress response is dynamic, synchronous and repeated assessments of psychological responses throughout the stress task may be more informative of participants’ stress perceptions during the task.35 Despite these limitations, the present findings demonstrate that differences in psychological reactivity to stress should not be overlooked when investigating neuroendocrine function among postmenopausal women. In conclusion, the present study noted that greater increases in negative affect and

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

Fang et al.

453

anxiety in response to a standardized laboratory stressor are associated with higher levels of cortisol, ACTH and DHEA in postmenopausal women. In light of the considerable data supporting an association between elevated DHEA and increased breast cancer risk, these findings point to one potential biological pathway via which stress exposure may be associated with breast cancer risk. Future studies exploring the role of stress in breast cancer risk should consider how variations in stress response may be associated with differential disease risk.

3.

4.

5.

Declaration of conflicting interest The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest. 6.

Funding This research was supported the National Institutes of Health (P30CA006927 and R03CA125770) and from the National Center for Research Resources (UL1RR024134).

7.

Acknowledgments The authors thank the study subjects for their participation in this study and the TSST evaluators for their assistance in implementing this project. They also thank the Fox Chase Cancer Center Biosample Repository Core Facility for its services.

8.

9.

References 1. Kudielka BM and Wu¨st S. Human models in acute and chronic stress: Assessing determinants of individual hypothalamus–pituitary– adrenal axis activity and reactivity. Stress 2010; 13: 1–14. 2. Claes SJ. Corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) in psychiatry: From stress

10.

to psychopathology. Ann Med 2004; 36: 50–61. Arvat E, Di Vito L, Lanfranco F, Maccario M, Baffoni C, Rossetto R, Aimaretti G, Camanni F and Ghigo E. Stimulatory effect of adrenocorticotropin on cortisol, aldosterone, and dehydroepiandrosterone secretion in normal humans: Dose–response study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2000; 85: 3141–3146. Kudielka BM, Buske-Kirschbaum A, Hellhammer DH, et al. HPA axis responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in healthy elderly adults, younger adults, and children: Impact of age and gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004; 29: 83–98. Dorgan JF, Longcope C, Stephenson HE Jr, et al. Relation of prediagnostic serum estrogen and androgen levels to breast cancer risk. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 1996; 5: 533–539. Dorgan JF, Stanczyk FZ, Longcope C, et al. Relationship of serum dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA), DHEA sulfate, and 5androstene-3 beta, 17 beta-diol to risk of breast cancer in postmenopausal women. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 1997; 6: 177–181. Gordon GB, Bush TL, Helzlsouer KJ, et al. Relationship of serum levels of dehydroepiandrosterone and dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate to the risk of developing postmenopausal breast cancer. Cancer Res 1990; 50: 3859–3862. Cauley JA, Lucas FL, Kuller LH, et al. Elevated serum estradiol and testosterone concentrations are associated with a high risk for breast cancer. Study of Osteoporotic Fractures Research Group. Ann Intern Med 1999; 130(Pt 1): 270–277. Missmer SA, Eliassen AH, Barbieri RL, et al. Endogenous estrogen, androgen, and progesterone concentrations and breast cancer risk among postmenopausal women. J Natl Cancer Inst 2004; 96: 1856–1865. Key T, Appleby P, Barnes I, et al. Endogenous sex hormones and breast cancer in postmenopausal women: Reanalysis of

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

454

11.

12.

13.

14.

15.

16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

21.

Journal of International Medical Research 42(2) nine prospective studies. J Natl Cancer Inst 2002; 94: 606–616. Longcope C. Adrenal and gonadal androgen secretion in normal females. Clin Endocrinol Metab 1986; 15: 213–228. van der Pompe G, Antoni MH and Heijnen CJ. Elevated basal cortisol levels and attenuated ACTH and cortisol responses to a behavioral challenge in women with metastatic breast cancer. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1996; 21: 361–374. Chrousos GP. The role of stress and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis in the pathogenesis of the metabolic syndrome: Neuro-endocrine and target tissue-related causes. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2000; 24(Suppl. 2): S50–S55. Cleary MP and Grossmann ME. Minireview: Obesity and breast cancer: The estrogen connection. Endocrinology 2009; 150: 2537–2542. Antonova L, Aronson K and Mueller CR. Stress and breast cancer: From epidemiology to molecular biology. Breast Cancer Res 2011; 13: 208. Butow PN, Hiller JE, Price MA, et al. Epidemiological evidence for a relationship between life events, coping style, and personality factors in the development of breast cancer. J Psychosom Res 2000; 49: 169–181. Helgesson O, Cabrera C, Lapidus L, et al. Self-reported stress levels predict subsequent breast cancer in a cohort of Swedish women. Eur J Cancer Prev 2003; 12: 377–381. Duijts SF, Zeegers MP and Borne BV. The association between stressful life events and breast cancer risk: A meta-analysis. Int J Cancer 2003; 107: 1023–1029. Roberts FD, Newcomb PA, Trentham-Dietz A, et al. Self-reported stress and risk of breast cancer. Cancer 1996; 77: 1089–1093. Price MA, Tennant CC, Butow PN, et al. The role of psychosocial factors in the development of breast carcinoma: Part II. Life event stressors, social support, defense style, and emotional control and their interactions. Cancer 2001; 91: 686–697. Kroenke CH, Hankinson SE, Schernhammer ES, et al. Caregiving stress, endogenous sex steroid hormone levels, and

22.

23.

24.

25.

26.

27.

28.

29.

30.

31.

breast cancer incidence. Am J Epidemiol 2004; 159: 1019–1027. Michael YL, Carlson NE, Chlebowski RT, et al. Influence of stressors on breast cancer incidence in the Women’s Health Initiative. Health Psychol 2009; 28: 137–146. Cohen S and Hamrick N. Stable individual differences in physiological response to stressors: Implications for stress-elicited changes in immune related health. Brain Behav Immun 2003; 17: 407–414. Tomaka J, Blascovich J, Kelsey RM, et al. Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. J Pers Soc Psychol 1993; 65: 248–260. Tomaka J, Blascovich J, Kibler J, et al. Cognitive and physiological antecedents of threat and challenge appraisal. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997; 73: 63–72. Izawa S, Sugaya N, Shirotsuki K, et al. Salivary dehydroepiandrosterone secretion in response to acute psychosocial stress and its correlations with biological and psychological changes. Biol Psychol 2008; 79: 294–298. Lennartsson AK, Kushnir MM, Bergquist J, et al. DHEA and DHEA-S response to acute psychosocial stress in healthy men and women. Biol Psychol 2012; 90: 143–149. Kudielka BM, Hellhammer DH and Wu¨st S. Why do we respond so differently? Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34: 2–18. Chong RY, Uhart M, McCaul ME, et al. Whites have a more robust hypothalamic– pituitary–adrenal axis response to a psychological stressor than blacks. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2008; 33: 246–254. Kelly MM, Tyrka AR, Anderson GM, et al. Sex differences in emotional and physiological responses to the Trier Social Stress Test. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2008; 39: 87–98. Yim IS, Quas JA, Cahill L, et al. Children’s and adults’ salivary cortisol responses to an identical psychosocial laboratory stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010; 35: 241–248.

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

Fang et al.

455

32. Giese-Davis J, Wilhelm FH, Conrad A, et al. Depression and stress reactivity in metastatic breast cancer. Psychosom Med 2006; 68: 675–683. 33. Harris DS, Reus VI, Wolkowitz OM, et al. Repeated psychological stress testing in stimulant-dependent patients. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2005; 29: 669–677. 34. Bagley SL, Weaver TL and Buchanan TW. Sex differences in physiological and affective responses to stress in remitted depression. Physiol Behav 2011; 104: 180–186. 35. Schlotz W, Kumsta R, Layes I, et al. Covariance between psychological and endocrine responses to pharmacological challenge and psychosocial stress: A question of timing. Psychosom Med 2008; 70: 787–796. 36. Al’Absi M, Bongard S, Buchanan T, et al. Cardiovascular and neuroendocrine adjustment to public speaking and mental arithmetic stressors. Psychophysiology 1997; 34: 266–275. 37. Lovallo WR, Pincomb GA, Brackett DJ, et al. Heart rate reactivity as a predictor of neuroendocrine responses to aversive and appetitive challenges. Psychosom Med 1990; 52: 17–26. 38. Oldehinkel AJ, Ormel J, Bosch NM, et al. Stressed out? Associations between perceived and physiological stress responses in adolescents: The TRAILS study. Psychophysiology 2011; 48: 441–452. 39. Singh A, Petrides JS, Gold PW, et al. Differential hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis reactivity to psychological and physical stress. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1999; 84: 1944–1948. 40. Uhart M, Chong RY, Oswald L, et al. Gender differences in hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis reactivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2006; 31: 642–652. 41. Childs E, Vicini LM and De Wit H. Responses to the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) in single versus grouped participants. Psychophysiology 2006; 43: 366–371. 42. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, et al. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety:

43.

44.

45.

46.

47.

48.

49.

50.

51.

52.

53.

Psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988; 56: 893–897. Beck AT and Steer RA. Beck Anxiety Inventory manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace and Company, 1993. Watson D, Clark LA and Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54: 1063–1070. Buchanan TW, al’Absi M and Lovallo WR. Cortisol fluctuates with increases and decreases in negative affect. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1999; 24: 227–241. Steptoe A, Gibson EL, Hamer M, et al. Neuroendocrine and cardiovascular correlates of positive affect measured by ecological momentary assessment and by questionnaire. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2007; 32: 56–64. Yildiz BO, Woods KS, Stanczyk F, et al. Stability of adrenocortical steroidogenesis over time in healthy women and women with polycystic ovary syndrome. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2004; 89: 5558–5562. Vogeser M, Engelhardt D and Jacob K. Comparison of two automated adrenocorticotropic hormone assays. Clin Chem 2000; 46: 1998–2000. Pruessner JC, Kirschbaum C, Meinlschmid G, et al. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus timedependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2003; 28: 916–931. Nolan S and Heinzen T. Statistics for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. New York, NY: Worth Publishers, 2011. Aschbacher K, Epel E, Wolkowitz OM, et al. Maintenance of a positive outlook during acute stress protects against pro-inflammatory reactivity and future depressive symptoms. Brain Behav Immun 2012; 26: 346–352. Dockray S and Steptoe A. Positive affect and psychobiological processes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010; 35: 69–75. Lindfors P and Lundberg U. Is low cortisol release an indicator of positive health? Stress Health 2002; 18: 153–160.

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

456

Journal of International Medical Research 42(2)

54. Lai JC, Evans PD, Ng SH, et al. Optimism, positive affectivity, and salivary cortisol. Br J Health Psychol 2005; 10(Pt. 4): 467–484. 55. Steptoe A, Dockray S and Wardle J. Positive affect and psychobiological processes relevant to health. J Pers 2009; 77: 1747–1776. 56. Steptoe A, O’Donnell K, Badrick E, et al. Neuroendocrine and inflammatory factors associated with positive affect in healthy men and women: The Whitehall II study. Am J Epidemiol 2008; 167: 96–102. 57. Steptoe A, Wardle J and Marmot M. Positive affect and health-related neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory processes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2005; 102: 6508–6512. 58. Brummett BH, Boyle SH, Kuhn CM, et al. Positive affect is associated with cardiovascular reactivity, norepinephrine level, and morning rise in salivary cortisol. Psychophysiology 2009; 46: 862–869. 59. Clow A, Thorn L, Evans P, et al. The awakening cortisol response: Methodological issues and significance. Stress 2004; 7: 29–37. 60. Adam EK, Hawkley LC, Kudielka BM, et al. Day-to-day dynamics of experience – cortisol associations in a population-based

61.

62.

63.

64.

65.

sample of older adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006; 103: 17058–17063. Kumsta R, Entringer S, Hellhammer DH, et al. Cortisol and ACTH responses to psychosocial stress are modulated by corticosteroid binding globulin levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2007; 32: 1153–1157. Pasquali R, Cantobelli S, Casimirri F, et al. The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis in obese women with different patterns of body fat distribution. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1993; 77: 341–346. Pasquali R, Biscotti D, Spinucci G, et al. Pulsatile secretion of ACTH and cortisol in premenopausal women: Effect of obesity and body fat distribution. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 1998; 48: 603–612. Therrien F, Drapeau V, Lalonde J, et al. Cortisol response to the Trier Social Stress Test in obese and reduced obese individuals. Biol Psychol 2010; 84: 325–329. Ginzburg K, Wrensch M, Rice T, et al. Breast cancer and psychosocial factors: Early stressful life events, social support, and well-being. Psychosomatics 2008; 49: 407–412.

Downloaded from imr.sagepub.com at UNIV OF BIRMINGHAM on June 4, 2015

Psychological reactivity to laboratory stress is associated with hormonal responses in postmenopausal women.

The present study examined associations between psychological reactivity and hormonal responses to a standardized laboratory stressor (Trier Social St...
206KB Sizes 0 Downloads 3 Views