570753 research-article2015

JIVXXX10.1177/0886260515570753Journal of Interpersonal ViolenceJensen et al.

Article

Sex Offender Populations and Clinical Efficacy: A Response to Rosky

Journal of Interpersonal Violence 1­–8 © The Author(s) 2015 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0886260515570753 jiv.sagepub.com

Todd M. Jensen, MSW1, Kevin Shafer, PhD2, C. Y. Roby, PhD3, and Jini L. Roby, MSW, JD2

Abstract We provide a brief response to a commentary submitted by Rosky in which he questions the rationale and methodological merits of our original study about full-disclosure polygraph outcome differences between juvenile and adult sex offenders. At the heart of Rosky’s substantive concerns is the premise that only research tying polygraphy outcomes to actual recidivism is useful or worthwhile. He also questions the overall utility and validity of polygraphy. We acknowledge and challenge these two points. Furthermore, many of the methodological concerns expressed by Rosky represent either a misunderstanding of our research question, study design, and sample, or a disregard for the explicit declarations we made with respect to our study limitations. Overall, it appears Rosky has accused us of not answering well a question we were not trying to ask. Our response addresses the key substantive and methodological concerns extended by Rosky and clarifies the actual aims and scope of our original study. We also argue that a calm, rational, and scientific discussion is the best approach to understanding how to improve clinical methods used in sex offender treatment. Keywords sexual assault, reporting/disclosure, sexual abuse, child abuse, treatment/ intervention, offenders 1University

of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, USA Young University, Provo, UT, USA 3C.Y. Roby, PhD, & Associates, Orem, UT, USA 2Brigham

Corresponding Author: Todd M. Jensen, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Tate-Turner-Kuralt Building, 325 Pittsboro Street RM 400, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA. Email: [email protected]

Downloaded from jiv.sagepub.com at Kungl Tekniska Hogskolan / Royal Institute of Technology on November 14, 2015

2

Journal of Interpersonal Violence 

The commentary by Rosky (in press) on our recent publication in the Journal of Interpersonal Violence exemplifies the passion of many scholars and clinicians in the field who are striving to move clinical interventions forward in fruitful directions for sex offender populations. We acknowledge the importance of, and strongly advocate for, competent sex offender treatment and for the use of clinical methods that are demonstrably helpful, not harmful, to the populations being served. An ongoing debate has emerged in the field that centers on how such methods can or should be proven helpful or harmful. We are hopeful that the discussion of our paper, Rosky’s comments, and our reply help illuminate this discussion further. We also believe that a calm, rational, and scientific discussion is best for understanding how to improve clinical methods used in sex offender treatment agencies. In regard to our study specifically, Rosky extended a number of criticisms regarding our study rationale, sample, analysis, and conclusions. In general, we feel that Rosky’s soaring rhetoric makes his rather specious argument sound far more convincing than it really is. In our reply, we will focus on the substance of his commentary, rather than the hyperbolic language he sprinkles throughout the text. For the sake of clarity and structure, we organized our responses into the following two sections: (a) substantive concerns and (b) methodological concerns.

Substantive Concerns At the heart of Rosky’s substantive concerns is the premise that only research tying polygraphy outcomes to one indicator of behavioral improvement— actual recidivism—is useful or worthwhile. Undoubtedly, strong evidence linking polygraphy outcomes to actual recidivism rates is an ideal and valuable outcome—a point we explicitly make in our paper. However, it seems strange to argue for a focus on a single outcome when there are likely numerous ways to assess the efficacy of sex offender treatment, including selfreports. Self-report measures in criminological contexts are not without precedence or substantiation (e.g., Piquero, Schubert, & Brame, 2014). Moreover, self-report measures are used in a sizable quantity of clinical contexts (e.g., Haroz, Ybarra, & Eaton, 2014; Head et al., 2013; Miilunpalo, Vuori, Oja, Pasanen, & Urponen, 1997). We do, however, acknowledge the serious nature of sexual offending, and the need to target reduction in recidivism—an additional point we make in our original paper. In any case, our study was not designed to resolve the long-lasting debate between proponents and opponents of polygraphy—nor do we claim anywhere in our paper that it does. We want to reiterate that point here: Our paper cannot and does not resolve the debate regarding the use of polygraphy in sex

Downloaded from jiv.sagepub.com at Kungl Tekniska Hogskolan / Royal Institute of Technology on November 14, 2015

Jensen et al.

3

offender treatment. Moreover, our study was not designed to assess any direct indicator of the clinical efficacy of polygraphy. Instead, our study was designed to answer one simple question: Do adult and juvenile offenders differ in their rate of passing sexual history disclosure polygraphs? It is unrealistic and unfounded to expect our study to do anything more. Had we set out to ask the research questions Rosky posited as being paramount and failed to adequately answer said questions, his concerns would be more reasonable. As nearly 50% of juvenile sex offender treatment agencies in the United States use polygraphy as a clinical tool (McGrath, Cumming, Burchard, Zeoli, & Ellerby, 2010), using available data to compare and contrast adult and juvenile offenders is relevant in our view and, apparently, in the view of the peer reviewers of our original manuscript. Given Rosky’s premise, it is also surprising that he suggested we include additional predictors in our logistic regression model. After all, Rosky stated that any assessment of polygraphy other than its direct link to recidivism is a “useless exercise.” However, we agree that future studies should assess the influence of race/ethnicity and offense type on polygraph outcomes—luxuries our current data set could not afford us. We explicitly mentioned these suggestions for future research in our discussion section. In addition, the arguments we extended in our discussion section were not meant to dissuade researchers from using recidivism rates as an outcome in future research. We believe that recidivism research is highly useful, and we believe that more research is needed in this area. Our arguments were simply made to pre-emptively address some of the concerns that polygraphy opponents might have about the merits of our study. We introduced these arguments primarily to nest our study within the current polygraphy debate, rather than ignoring it. Unfortunately, our data did not include indicators of offender recidivism, rendering impossible the inclusion of this indicator in our analysis. This represents a common limitation among studies in the polygraphy literature. In regard to Rosky’s general concerns about polygraphy, including the potential for false-positive or false-negative results, we offer several responses. First, each offender who initially failed his or her polygraph in our study admitted to not being truthful and eventually passed a later examination. The only possible exceptions were those incarcerated as a result of offenses completed between the initial failed polygraph and the completion of a second polygraph. Second, Rosky noted a concern about offenders being placed into treatments or receiving sanctions as a result of false-positive polygraph outcomes. The examinees in our study were already in treatment, and the state in which treatment was conducted only required that treatment progress be delayed as a result of a failed full-disclosure polygraph. Third, all polygraph examinations were conducted by an examiner with a proper state

Downloaded from jiv.sagepub.com at Kungl Tekniska Hogskolan / Royal Institute of Technology on November 14, 2015

4

Journal of Interpersonal Violence 

license, decades of experience in conducting examinations, and time served on the licensing board for Deception Detection Examiners. Fourth, Rosky stated, “It seems reasonable that some passed merely because they weren’t asked the right questions about their sexual histories” (p. XX). In actuality, all offenders were asked to complete a very detailed pre-polygraph sexual history questionnaire, and the polygraph examiner asked whether the offender had committed any sexual offenses that were not included in his or her questionnaire. Thus, regardless of age or history, each polygraph examination was suitable for each offender’s history. Again, disclosed sexual histories were not used to further sanction or charge offenders with additional crimes, but were used as a state-mandated treatment tool. Moreover, offender risk assessments provided by instruments such as the Static 99 are responsive to past sexual offenses, and polygraph examinations can help verify offender sexual histories. If a more accurate method became available for such verification, treatment providers would likely embrace this new method.

Methodological Concerns Many of the methodological concerns expressed by Rosky represent either a misunderstanding of our research question, study design, and sample, or a disregard for the explicit declarations we made with respect to our study limitations. He specifically commented on the potential nonindependence of observations, our measure for age, and our sample. We address each of these concerns in turn. In terms of the potential nonindependence of our study observations, we mentioned in our study that hierarchical linear modeling (HLM), or multilevel modeling, was not necessary, as our data did not have a hierarchical structure (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Specifically, only a single measurement occasion was analyzed for each participant, and all participants were nested within the same polygraph examiner. Had there been multiple measurement occasions, and had there been multiple polygraph examiners, then measurement occasions would have been nested within participants, and participants would have been nested within examiners (Heck & Thomas, 2009; Hox, 2010; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Rosky’s suggestion is akin to asking an education researcher to use HLM in an effort to model teacher and school effects in a sample of individuals who have the same teacher and share the same school—it is simply useless because there is no higher-level variation to estimate. In response to our rationale for not using HLM, Rosky stated, “not being able to do a particular analysis to account for certain types of variability does not mean that type of variability doesn’t exist and doesn’t affect the analyses” (p. XX). In

Downloaded from jiv.sagepub.com at Kungl Tekniska Hogskolan / Royal Institute of Technology on November 14, 2015

5

Jensen et al.

general, this is true, but this statement is not accurate as it pertains to our study and the partition of outcome variance to between- and within-examiner components. For instance, the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) is used to assess the so-called cluster effect of a particular set of data (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). The ICC is calculated using the following equation: ρ=

τ00 σ2 + τ00

,

where τ00 represents the between-group variance (i.e., between examiners), and σ2 represents the within-group (i.e., within examiners) variance. Because our data were derived from a single polygraph examiner, there was no between-group variance to estimate. Thus, we were left with withingroup, or within-examiner, variance only. This equation both illustrates the nonclustered nature of our data and disconfirms Rosky’s claim that not having multiple polygraph examiners increases the likelihood of nonindependent observations within our data set. For the sake of additional precaution, we assessed the extent to which the date of the polygraph might have influenced our model results by including a variable for time in the logistic regression. The substantive results of this model were the same. We also addressed the influence of other potential unobserved sources of data clustering by using robust standard errors. Results from the robust logistic regression yielded the same substantive findings. In summary, there was no variation in agency, locale, or examiner to estimate in our original study. The suggestion that we use HLM results from either a lack of knowledge about HLM or a misunderstanding about our data. Rosky also expressed concerns regarding our measure for participant age. Specifically, he argued that categorical groupings of age potentially overlook individual developmental characteristics and nuances. This is precisely why we used a continuous measure of biological age in our logistic regression model. The categorical grouping for age was used only in our preliminary bivariate analysis. Although our models did not account for each participant’s developmental characteristics, this criterion seems unrealistic and incongruent with the broader polygraphy literature. Rosky also pointed out that our sample was nonrandom. Indeed, we used a nonprobability sample. In fact, we stated the following in our paper: “Our participants were not gathered through random sampling, and thus our results yielded neither national representation nor generalizability—a common issue within the sex offender literature” (Jensen, Shafer, Roby, & Roby, 2014, p.13). Thus, we are being criticized for something that we readily admitted ourselves. We did note, however, that the unique clinical nature of our data

Downloaded from jiv.sagepub.com at Kungl Tekniska Hogskolan / Royal Institute of Technology on November 14, 2015

6

Journal of Interpersonal Violence 

might be useful for understanding the implications of our results—a point that we will not concede. Rosky also labeled our sample “small,” with potential adverse consequences for the statistical power of our analytical model. Our sample was comprised of 238 adult and 86 juvenile offenders (total N = 324). In most statistical contexts in which maximum likelihood estimation is used, a sample of 300 cases or more is considered adequate (refer to Kline, 2011, pp. 11-12). This is particularly true when one is analyzing a very simple model, such as ours. Furthermore, because our logistic regression model used a continuous form of age, our statistical power was not diminished due to the relatively lower number of juveniles in our sample.

Conclusion Overall, it appears Rosky has accused us of not answering well a question we were not trying to ask. From that perspective, he is right: Our study does not do a good job answering a question about recidivism because our paper never attempted to focus on that question. Instead, our study was designed to answer a simple research question in a substantive area ripe for additional research. Some of the methodological concerns Rosky pointed out appear to represent a misunderstanding of statistical principles or the data we analyzed. Many of his other concerns were explicitly addressed in the original manuscript. Despite our transparency, Rosky decided to submit a paper in which he reiterated the limitations of our study. As mentioned previously, we also alluded to the ongoing polygraphy debate not to resolve it, but to provide context for our study. Regardless of any disagreements Rosky might have with our study, we respect and value his skepticism. Skepticism within the scientific community is a necessary ingredient for continual empirical and clinical growth and discovery. In that spirit, we are excited to see new polygraph research emerge in the future. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References Haroz, E. E., Ybarra, M. L., & Eaton, W. W. (2014). Psychometric evaluation of a self-report scale to measure adolescent depression: The CESDR-10 in two

Downloaded from jiv.sagepub.com at Kungl Tekniska Hogskolan / Royal Institute of Technology on November 14, 2015

Jensen et al.

7

national adolescent samples in the United States. Journal of Affective Disorders, 158, 154-160. Head, J., Stansfeld, S. A., Ebmeier, K. P., Geddes, J. R., Allan, C. L., Lewis, G., & Kivimäki, M. (2013). Use of self-administered instruments to assess psychiatric disorders in older people: Validity of the General Health Questionnaire, the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale and the self-completion version of the revised Clinical Interview Schedule. Psychological Medicine, 43, 2649-2656. Heck, R., & Thomas, S. (2009). An introduction to multilevel modeling techniques. New York, NY: Routledge. Hox, J. (2010). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. Jensen, T., Shafer, K., Roby, C., & Roby, J. (2014). Sexual history disclosure polygraph outcomes: Do juvenile and adult sex offenders differ? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 30, 928-944. doi:10.1177/0886260514539766 Kline, R. (2011). Principles and practice of structure equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. McGrath, R. J., Cumming, G. F., Burchard, B. L., Zeoli, S., & Ellerby, L. (2010). Current practices and emerging trends in sexual abuser management: The safer society 2009 North American survey. Brandon, VT: Safer Society Press. Miilunpalo, S., Vuori, I., Oja, P., Pasanen, M., & Urponen, H. (1997). Self-rated health status as a health measure: The predictive value of self-reported health status on the use of physician services and on mortality in the working-age population. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 50, 517-528. Piquero, A. R., Schubert, C. A., & Brame, R. (2014). Comparing official and selfreport records of offending across gender and race/ethnicity in a longitudinal study of serious youthful offenders. Journal of Research in Crime & Delinquency, 51, 526-556. Raudenbush, S., & Bryk, A. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rosky, J. (in press). More polygraph futility: A comment on Jensen, Shafer, Roby, and Roby (2014). Journal of Interpersonal Violence.

Author Biographies Todd M. Jensen, MSW, is a doctoral student in the School of Social Work at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. His primary research interests include stepfamily functioning and intervention, stepchild perspectives, family resilience, family transitions across the life course, and social work practice. Kevin Shafer, PhD, is an assistant professor in the School of Social Work at Brigham Young University. His research interests include men’s mental health, social demography, research methods, statistics for the social sciences, social stratification and marriage, divorce, remarriage, and immigrant families in the United States.

Downloaded from jiv.sagepub.com at Kungl Tekniska Hogskolan / Royal Institute of Technology on November 14, 2015

8

Journal of Interpersonal Violence 

C. Y. Roby, PhD, is a clinical psychologist with decades of experience in forensic treatment settings. He specializes in court-ordered sex offender treatment, and stands as a well-respected and valued expert in the sex offender treatment community, particularly within the Intermountain West Area. Jini L. Roby, MSW, JD, is a professor in the School of Social Work at Brigham Young University. With a strong presence in international settings, her research interests include international child welfare with a focus on orphan care and supportive parenting policies.

Downloaded from jiv.sagepub.com at Kungl Tekniska Hogskolan / Royal Institute of Technology on November 14, 2015

Sex Offender Populations and Clinical Efficacy: A Response to Rosky.

We provide a brief response to a commentary submitted by Rosky in which he questions the rationale and methodological merits of our original study abo...
346KB Sizes 1 Downloads 5 Views