Brief Report Cephalalgia 2014, Vol. 34(7) 548–553 ! International Headache Society 2014 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0333102413517774 cep.sagepub.com

The perception of time in childhood migraine Carmelo M Vicario1, Mariangela Gulisano2, Davide Martino3 and Renata Rizzo2

Abstract Introduction: Migraine is a syndrome rather common in children. Recent results have accounted evidence of altered activity corresponding to the supplemental motor area in this pediatric population. Given the role of this region in the motor timing of supra-second temporal intervals, we hypothesized that time representation is abnormal in childhood migraine. Subjects and methods: We compared time-processing abilities between 11 children with migraine and 11 age-matched healthy children, employing a time reproduction task in which subjects actively reproduced different supra-second temporal intervals. Results: We show that children with migraine are less accurate (overestimate) and more variable in reproducing time intervals. Conclusion: Results are discussed on the basis of the recent evidence about abnormal cortical and neurochemical activity in childhood migraine, but also in relation to a possible alteration of spatial attention mechanisms in this clinical population. The low number of patients examined represents a limit of this study and indicates that further investigation is needed. Keywords Time processing, migraine, childhood Date received: 20 August 2013; revised: 5 October 2013; 13 November 2013; accepted: 14 November 2013

Introduction Migraine is a recurrent headache disorder manifesting in attacks lasting four to 72 hours, typically characterized by unilateral location, pulsating quality, moderatesevere intensity, aggravation by routine physical activity, and association with nausea and/or photophobia and phonophobia (International Classification of Headache Disorders, second edition (ICHD-II)). The prevalence rate of migraine before puberty is between 3% and 24.5% (1), and younger children may commonly report pain on both sides of the head. Children with migraine were found to exhibit mild abnormalities in several cognitive domains. Abnormalities in the responding latency and source activation patterns were reported, with the intriguing finding of significantly higher odds of activation in the supplementary motor area (SMA) in children with migraine compared to age-matched control subjects (2). This abnormal activation pattern of the SMA during the execution of motor tasks raises the possibility that the spectrum

of cognitive abnormalities observed in pediatric migraine might include also a complex domain such as time processing, particularly if related to motor tasks (motor timing). A recent meta-analysis (3) on the functional anatomy of time processing highlighted the SMA as a crucial area involved in the detection of supra-second temporal intervals. The processing of supra-second temporal intervals is considered a cognitively controlled timing task, since it requires the

1

School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Australia Dipartimento di Scienze Mediche e Pediatriche, Catania University, Italy 3 Neuroscience & Trauma Centre, Barts and The London School of Medicine and Dentistry, Queen Mary University London, UK 2

Corresponding author: Carmelo M Vicario, School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD, 4072, Australia. Email: [email protected]

549

Vicario et al. Table 1. Attack frequency was divided into high (HF; 10–14 attacks a month), intermediate (IF; from five to nine attacks per month) and low frequency (LF; from less than once a month to four attacks per month), and chronic migraine (CM) for the patient who presented 15 or more migraine attacks per month. Subjects

Diagnosis

Scalp localization

Frequency (per week)

Intensity of pain

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

Migraine with aura Migraine Migraine Migraine Migraine with aura Migraine Migraine with aura Migraine with aura Tension-type headache Migraine Tension-type headache

Right temporal Left temporal Left frontal Left frontal Left frontal Right temporal Right temporal Left temporal Bilateral temporal Right frontal Bilateral central

3 2 5 3 2 2 3 3 4 2 2

MoP MP MP MP MP MoP MoP MoP SP MP SP

HF IF CM HF IF IF HF HF HF IF IF

Patients were asked to rate pain intensity on a three-level graduated scale: 1) mild pain (MP), allowing the subject to continue his daily activities; 2) moderate pain (MoP), leading to interruption of the patient’s activities; and 3) severe pain (SP), forcing the child to go to bed.

explicit orientation of attentional sources toward the duration of stimuli lasting more than one second (4,5). Anagnostou and Mitsikostas (6) performed the first study in adults with migraine (during the interictal period) by using a task that involved time processing of auditory stimuli. Results show temporal abnormalities, although performance has been explained in relation to co-existing depressive symptoms. More recently, time processing has been explored in adult migraineurs (interictal period) using a motor timing task (7). The authors documented timing abnormalities selectively for the sub-second interval range. Here, we expand this issue by testing temporal processing in children with pediatric migraine and healthy volunteers. Given the reported abnormal SMA activation pattern (2), patients were submitted to a motor timing task. We predict a worse performance for our clinical sample compared to controls.

per week for all patients (Table 1). All children were tested during the interictal period. We compared their performance with that of a control group of 11 right-handed, age-matched healthy volunteers (six males and five females, age 8.1  0.28). Children with migraine were recruited at the Department of Pediatrics, Ospedale Policlinico, University of Catania, Italy. Children without migraine were recruited from a primary school in Palermo, Italy. Children with Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition, text revision (DSMIV-R) diagnoses of co-morbid neurological and psychiatric disorders, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), obsessive-compulsive disorder, anxiety, depressive and conduct disorders, were excluded (10) (see Supporting document). The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Department of Pediatrics, Ospedale Policlinico, University of Catania, Italy, and written consent was obtained from all participants and their adult caregivers.

Subjects and methods Participants

Procedure

Eleven right-handed children with migraine (six males and five females, age 11.272.7) were diagnosed according to the criteria of the International Headache Society (8). Patients also met ICHD-III (beta) (9); according to this classification our sample was divided as follows: migraine with aura group (n ¼ 4); migraine without aura group (n ¼ 5) and tension-type headache group (n ¼ 2); the tension type group satisfied only the criteria for ‘‘frequent headache.’’ Mean ictal frequency was 2.81  0.98 attacks

We used a version of the time reproduction task previously used by our group (11,12). Subjects sat at a distance of 50 cm opposite the monitor configured to a refresh rate of 60 Hz, and fixated on a black cross of 0.2 degrees in diameter, centrally located on the screen. After 500 ms, the black circle (test stimulus; size: 0.8 degrees  0.1 degrees) appeared in the same location of the fixating cross; after a specified period, it disappeared. Immediately after the black circle disappeared, subjects were instructed to reproduce the interval they

550

Cephalalgia 34(7)

had just perceived by pressing the space bar on the computer keyboard with the index finger of their dominant (right) hand. During such time they fixated on a white screen. When they had judged that the same amount of time had elapsed, they had to release the space bar. At this point a new fixation cross immediately appeared on the computer screen to alert the arrival of a new trial (Figure 1). We chose to challenge subjects in the reproduction of five different time intervals (1500, 1600, 1700, 1800,

Ready signal 500 msec

Sequence direction

Stimulus reproduction Stimulus duration range 1500-1900 msec Ready signal 500 msec

Figure 1. Experimental procedure. Example of typical event trials for time reproduction.

1900 ms) in order to minimize the chance of a learning process throughout the performance of the task. The total number of trials was 50 (10 trials  five intervals). Stimulus durations were presented in a randomized order.

Results Trials that fell three standard deviations (SD) above or below each individual mean for each experimental condition were excluded as outliers. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) for repeated measures on accuracy detected a significant main effect of group (migraine vs control) factor on the time reproduction task (F(1,20) ¼ 4.77, p ¼ 0.040, 2 ¼ 0.192): Migraine participants overestimated durations with respect to control participants. Likewise we also detected a main effect of duration (1500–1900 ms) factor (F(4,80) ¼ 7.94, p < 0.001, 2 ¼ 0.284). No significant result was reported for the interaction term group  duration (F(4,80) ¼ 0.71, p ¼ 0.584, 2 ¼ 0.034; see Figure 2). The coefficient of variation (CV, SD/mean response time) was also analyzed as a measure of performance variability. No significant main effect of group was detected (F(1,20) ¼ 1.53, p < 0.229, 2 ¼ 0.071). However, we detected a main effect of duration (F(4,80) ¼ 4.77, p ¼ 0.001, 2 ¼ 0.192) and of

1900

No migraine Migraine

1800

Response duration (msec)

1700 1600 1500 1400 1300 1200 1100 1000 1500

1600

1700

1800

1900

Figure 2. Response duration: Duration means of the pressure on the space bar while reproducing supra-second intervals for migraine and no-migraine participants. The vertical bars indicate standard error of the mean.

551

Vicario et al.

0.40

No migraine Migraine

Coefficient of variation

0.36 ∗

0.32

0.28

0.24

0.20

0.16 1500

1600

1700

1800

1900

Figure 3. Coefficient of variation (CV): This parameter indicates the group variability while reproducing supra-second intervals for migraine and no-migraine participants. The * indicates significant post-hoc comparison differences. The vertical bars indicate standard error of the mean.

group  duration interaction (F(4,80) ¼ 17.01, p < 0.001, 2 ¼ 0.459). Post-hoc analysis (t test Bonferroni corrected) showed that patients were significantly more variable than controls on 1900 ms interval reproduction (p ¼ 0.019). No significant difference was detected for the other intervals (p ¼ 1.00; Figure 3). No within-group difference was detected comparing patients with predominantly left-sided pain compared to patients with predominantly right-sided pain (t ¼ 0.23; p ¼ 0.817). Finally, we performed a correlation analysis between the average accuracy and variability scores and participants’ age. We observed a trend for a negative correlation between temporal overestimation and age (r ¼ 0.579, p ¼ 0.061), and for a positive correlation between CV and age (r ¼ 0.56, p ¼ 0.067). No such trend was observed in the control group (accuracy: r ¼ 0.439, p ¼ 0.204; CV: r ¼ –0.45, p ¼ 0.191).

Discussion Research on time perception in the pediatric population is scanty if compared with that available for the adult population. The study of time processing in children affected by neurological and cognitive disorders has involved participants affected by ADHD (13), Tourette syndrome (14), dyscalculia (15) and inherited conditions (16). Our study expands this field by

providing the first case-control study in pediatric migraine. In particular, we chose a classical motor timing task, based on the evidence (2) of a deranged activity within cortical regions modulating motor control, such as the SMA. According to the results of Zhang et al. (7), we found that children with migraine overestimate temporal intervals. However, contrary to what was claimed by these authors, we detected an alteration of the supra-second temporal domain. A possible explanation for the difference between the current result and that reported by Zhang et al. (7) could refer to the fact that these authors tested timing performance for durations longer (three to five seconds) than those considered in our study (1500–1900 ms). Moreover, we studied pediatric populations, which are known to differ from adult in timing performance (17). The result of CV analysis has also shown that children with migraine showed more variable results in reproducing durations, although this difference was significant only for the longer temporal interval (1900 ms). The exclusion of severe cognitive deficits potentially able to influence the timing performance of our clinical sample, e.g. working memory and sustained attention, makes a role of cognitive profile in our participants’ performance unlikely. Moreover, our participants were not affected by depressive symptoms, another potential confounder for timing performance (6). Nevertheless, our migraine sample was heterogeneous

552 for type of migraine, pain location, severity and frequency of attacks, hindering the identification of which, among these features, is primarily responsible for timing performance. From a neurophysiological perspective, different factors can be called into play, beyond the role of SMA (10), to explain the observed abnormality in time processing. Dysfunctional neurotransmission might be involved. In a recent review (18) it is postulated that migraine attacks could be characterized by an ictal dopamine release in subjects with dopamine receptor hypersensitivity secondary to a chronic dopaminergic deficit synergistic to serotoninergic impairment. Given the role of both these neurotransmitters in temporal

Cephalalgia 34(7) processing (19), we cannot exclude their influence on timing performance. Alternatively, the alteration of brain arousal documented in this clinical population by previous works (20) may also be involved, given that increase was proposed to explain the relative overestimation of time intervals longer than a second (20). Our study bears limitations, including the relatively low number of patients, a limited exploration of timeprocessing abilities exclusively to the supra-second range, and clinical heterogeneity. This notwithstanding, we believe it creates the rationale for more extensive investigation of this complex cognitive domain in pediatric migraine.

Clinical implications . Accurate temporal estimations play a critical role in planning action and formulating decisions in everyday life. The evidence of altered timing functions in childhood migraine is relevant to understand the impact played by this disorder on cognitive development. Particularly, abnormalities of executive functioning (EF) development may negatively affect overall academic and social functioning. A recent work has discussed the strict relationship between EF development (attention, working memory and inhibitory control) and the progression of the level of sophistication of time-keeping skills. Accordingly, the study of time processing in childhood migraineurs is timely, since it might be a reflection of a broader abnormality of EF development. Funding This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest None declared.

References 1. Lipton RB and Bigal ME. Migraine: Epidemiology, impact, and risk factors for progression. Headache 2005; 45(Suppl 1): S3–S13. 2. Guo X, Xiang J, Wang Y, et al. Aberrant neuromagnetic activation in the motor cortex in children with acute migraine: A magnetoencephalography study. PLoS One 2012; 7: e50095. 3. Wiener M, Turkeltaub P and Coslett HB. The image of time: A voxel-wise meta-analysis. Neuroimage 2010; 49: 1728–1740. 4. Lewis PA and Miall RC. Remembering the time: A continuous clock. Trends Cogn Sci 2006; 10: 401–406. 5. Vicario CM. Cognitively controlled timing and executive functions develop in parallel? A glimpse on childhood research. Front Behav Neurosci 2013; 10(7): 146. 6. Anagnostou E and Mitsikostas DD. Time perception in migraine sufferers: An experimental matched-pairs study. Cephalalgia 2005; 25: 60–67.

7. Zhang J, Wang G, Jiang Y, et al. The study of time perception in migraineurs. Headache 2012; 52: 1483–1498. 8. Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society. Classification and diagnostic criteria for headache disorders, cranial neuralgias and facial pain. Cephalalgia 1988; 8: 1–96. 9. The International Classification of Headache Disorders, third edition (beta version). Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society (IHS). Cephalalgia 2013; 33: 629–808. 10. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV), 4th edn. Washington, DC: APA, 1994. 11. Vicario CM, Bonnı´ S and Koch G. Left hand dominance affects supra-second time processing. Front Integr Neurosci 2011; 5: 65. 12. Vicario CM, Martino D and Koch G. Temporal accuracy and variability in the left and right posterior parietal cortex. Neuroscience 2013; 245: 121–128. 13. Walg M, Oepen J and Prior H. Adjustment of time perception in the range of seconds and milliseconds: The nature of time-processing alterations in children with ADHD. J Atten Disord. Epub ahead of print 30 July 2012. PMID: 22851208. 14. Vicario CM, Martino D, Spata F, et al. Time processing in children with Tourette’s syndrome. Brain Cogn 2010; 73: 28–34. 15. Vicario CM, Rappo G, Pepi A, et al. Temporal processing in children with developmental dyscalculia. Dev Neuropsychol 2012; 37: 636–652.

Vicario et al. 16. Vicario CM, Yates MJ and Nicholls ME. Shared deficits in space, time, and quantity processing in childhood genetic disorders. Front Psychol 2013; 4: 43. 17. Droit-Volet S. Time perception in children: A neurodevelopmental approach. Neuropsychologia 2013; 51: 220–234.

553 18. Barbanti P, Fofi L, Aurilia C, et al. Dopaminergic symptoms in migraine. Neurol Sci 2013; 34: 1: S67–S70. 19. Rammsayer T. Dopaminergic and serotoninergic influence on duration discrimination and vigilance. Pharmacopsychiatry 1989; 22: 39–43. 20. Aurora SK and Wilkinson F. The brain is hyperexcitable in migraine. Cephalalgia 2007; 27: 1442–1453.

The perception of time in childhood migraine.

Migraine is a syndrome rather common in children. Recent results have accounted evidence of altered activity corresponding to the supplemental motor a...
321KB Sizes 1 Downloads 0 Views