Archimedes

Towards evidence based medicine for paediatricians Edited by Bob Phillips Before. And after In the world of non-randomised studies, there are bucketloads of variants, a common one that we see is the ‘before and after study’. This is, on the face of it, a sensible approach. Do your ‘thing’, then change stuff, do the ‘other thing’. Monitor something important you hope to change, and then if it does, you have some evidence of benefit. Except for most before and after studies it’s not quite like that. They are rarely conducted prospectively, but usually because Something is going on—rising infection rates, loads of Kawasaki diagnoses, increasing ‘do not attend’ rates—and Something Must Be Done. So it is. And then the worrying thing falls back down again. This is then noted, and the notes are trawled, and the Something That Was Done is given a point at which before was before, and after was after. Can you see any flaws? For a start, there’s random fluctuation, which is a particular problem with rare occurrences. After a blip, you’re likely to get things settling down (‘regression to the mean’). Then there’s the whole before/after divide—things are rarely Done in one clean sweep, and there’s a period of fluff and movement. And there’s also the idea that when you start Doing Something, all sorts of other things happen too. (Take bare below the elbows. There’s little direct evidence that the movement of cloth from off the wrist makes a difference to infections. What it probably does is signal something; a desire to wash hands a lot, to encourage others to do so, to isolate infections quickly and to move to improve things for all. It might be claimed that the Thing was rolling up of sleeves, but the sleeves probably didn’t do the dirty work.) And then there’s publication bias. If you did Something and it didn’t work, how likely are you to report it? For instance, if with VIP-induced diarrhoea you commenced octreotide and the poo settled, you’d probably write it up. But if you commenced racecontradil and next to nothing happened, no one’s going to be encouraging you to submit an abstract to the Spring Meeting. Now in all this scepticism, there are some things you may be able to hold on to. ▸ Really, really big effects—things with greater than fivefold change—are likely to be real. ▸ Actions which are truly before/after, or other discontinuities, for example, when a drug is banned from use, and the outcome is consistently monitored in both time frames. ▸ Evidence of clear difference in the smoothed trends between the two periods, or differences where the period of change is removed from the analysis (taking out the ‘blip’ that might have driven change) are weakly more convincing. You may have really liked this study design before. But after? Correspondence to Dr Bob, Phillips, Centre for Reviews and Dissemination, University of York, York YO10 5DD, UK;[email protected], bob. [email protected] Competing interests None. Provenance and peer review Commissioned; internally peer reviewed. Received 22 December 2013 Accepted 23 December 2013

▸ http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2013-304973 ▸ http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2013-305805 Arch Dis Child 2014;99:390. doi:10.1136/archdischild-2013-305898 390

Arch Dis Child April 2014 Vol 99 No 4

Towards evidence based medicine for paediatricians.

Towards evidence based medicine for paediatricians. - PDF Download Free
79KB Sizes 0 Downloads 3 Views