Cocaine-Induced

Paranoia

Sally

L. Satel,

Objective: The aim transient cocaine-induced cocaine-dependent

undergoing The

men

men

substance were

measures

Results:

The combined strongly

who

had

been

using

treatment;

with

with

on the Perceptual a history

may

experience (Am

J

be at higher

risk

for

power users

development

Scale

week

and

the Magical

and

Ideation

Scale

and Magical The

90.0%, transient than

were

paranoia.

offunctional

paranoia.

were 80.0%, who experience ofpsychosis

per

cocaine-induced

the development

Aberration

who experience subjects were 20

Scale,

psychosis.

Ideation

sensitivity,

Scale specific-

88.9%, and 81.8%, paranoia while in-

cocaine

users

who

do not

paranoia. Psychiatry

1 991

; 148:1

708-1

71 1)

I

is web! known that drugs of abuse can produce both transient and persistent psychotic symptoms in mdividuabs without histories of primary psychotic illness (1-3). Whether such individuals possess an underlying predisposition to psychosis in the absence of pharmacobogic stress is unknown. Evidence of such a predisposition has been provided by several studies. Tsuang et al. (4) studied consecutively admitted substance users with and without psychosis. Use of habbucinogens and cannabinoids was highly prevalent in the 72 substance abusers who developed psychoses; their premombid histories and symptoms were comparable to those of a group of schizophrenic patients. By contrast, a comparison group of 30 substance abusers who did not develop psychosis tended to use drugs without psychotomimetic properties-barbiturates, benzodiazepines, and opiates. Bowers and Swigar (5) examined 95 patients hospitalized for acute psychosis; 60 had used hallucinogens within 3 years of admission. Among the male subjects, those with positive family histories of major mental illness appeared capable of becoming psychotic with relatively small amounts of hallucinogen, whereas those without such histories required greater exposure. In a 6-year follow-up of 1 1 amphetamine usems who were asymptomatic at the time of initial treatment, McLellan et al. (6) diagnosed seven as psychotic; none of the opiate abusers or alcoholics was psychotic

Received Jan. 2, 1991; revision received May 15, 1991; accepted June 14, 1991. From the Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, and the Yale Psychiatric Institute, New Haven, Conn., and the Psychiatry Service, West Haven VA Medical Center. Address reprint requests to Dr. Satel, Psychiatry Service 1 1 6A, VA Medical Center, 950 Campbell Ave., West Haven, CT 06516.

1708

Ph.D.

5 g of cocaine

of cocaine-induced

and positive and negative predictive respectively. Conclusions: Heavy cocaine

Proneness

individuals Method: The

binge-limited

to precede

ity,

toxicated

S. Edell,

than

Aberration

thought

scores

more

halfreported

the Perceptual

ofsymptoms

correlated

and William

M.D.,

this study was to determine whether paranoia are vulnerable to psychosis.

abuse

assessed

self-report were

of

and Psychosis

at follow-up. These investigators and others (1, 7, 8) have speculated that subthreshold psychotic symptoms may have actually influenced the subjects’ selection of drugs and that symptoms associated with psychotic disorders may have been present but below the limit of clinical detection at the time drug use began. Cocaine use has been associated with paranoia, but not all users develop this symptom despite prolonged and heavy exposure (9-1 1). In our earlier study (12) of cocaine-dependent men, none of whom had other axis I diagnoses, 34 (68%) reported paranoid episodes limited to their periods of cocaine use. The paranoia did not extend beyond the “crash,” or hypersomnolent phase, of early cocaine abstinence. The paranoia had developed after an average of 3 years’ use. Importantly, the subjects reporting paranoia did not differ significantly from those not reporting paranoia with respect to use characteristics such as route of administration, lifetime duration of use, cumulative exposure to cocaine, amount of cocaine consumed in month of admission, or concurrent use of other drugs. These findings suggest that development of paranoia is not simply the result of exceeding a threshold of use and that affected individuals may be predisposed to this drug-induced state. Chapman et al. have developed a series of objective scales that attempt to identify individuals at higher than normal risk for psychosis. Two of the most promising scales measure perceptual aberrations (1 3) and magical ideation (14). These scales ascertain, through self-report, experiences that are characteristic of nascent psychotic states. Numerous published studies (15-20) have documented the reliability and validity of these instruments. Unlike the standard approach of studying the

so

Am

J

Psychiatry

1 48:1 2, December

1991

SALLY

relatives of psychotic patients, which may generate an unrepresentative and restricted risk sample, this strategy measures symptoms that are thought to precede psychosis and allows for the study of a greater variety of individuals with psychotic diathesis (16). In the present study we attempted to answer the question of whether individuals who display transient cocaine-induced paranoid symptoms are predisposed to psychosis. We hypothesized that scores on the scales of psychosis proneness would discriminate between mdividuabs who display such paranoid symptoms and those who do not.

METhOD

Subjects The subjects were recruited from patients consecutiveby admitted to the inpatient and outpatient units of a VA substance abuse treatment program. A total of 32 patients were screened with the Cocaine Experience Questionnaire (description to follow). The first 10 patients who reported binge-limited (transient) paranoid experiences during cocaine intoxication and the first 10 who denied such experiences were included. It was necessary to exclude 12 patients to achieve this distribution because patients who reported paranoia were overrepresented among the heavy cocaine users (12, 21). The subjects were assigned to groups blindly, i.e., the subjects were determined to be psychosis prone by one author (W.S.E.), who had no knowledge of the specific subjective response to cocaine, and the response to cocaine was determined by the other author (S.L.S.). The inclusion criteria for the study were as follows. 1 . Primary DSM-III-R axis I diagnosis of cocaine depcndcncc and a minimum of 3 years of continuous use of at least approximately S g of cocaine per week. The latter requirement was based on our earlier work (12), which demonstrated that development of paranoia in vulnerable individuals occurs after an average of 3 years’ use of at least S g of cocaine per week. 2. No concurrent DSM-III-R substance dependence diagnosis or other axis I affective or psychotic disorder. A patient who used another substance was included if the substance was used to modify the acute effects of cocaine and if such use did not take place in the absence of cocaine intoxication. 3. No prior psychiatric hospitalizations except for detoxification and rehabilitation. 4. Absence of psychotic symptoms or paranoia outside the context of cocaine use. S. Absence of DSM-III-R axis II diagnoses of schizotypal on schizoid personality disorder. 6. Negative family history of psychosis in first-degree relatives. The study was designed so that the final group of 20 male subjects was equally divided between those reporting cocaine-induced paranoia and those not reporting such symptoms. All those invited agreed to participate.

Am

J

Psychiatry

I 48:1 2, December

1991

U. SATEL

AND

WILLIAM

S. EDEUL

Instruments The Cocaine Experience Questionnaire contains 58 items and assesses methods and patterns of cocaine use and the extent and nature of the paranoid experiences associatedwith use. Family history of functional psychotic symptoms was elicited during administration of the questionnaire. The subjects were carefully instructed to distinguish adaptive hypervigilance or anxiety in high-risk situations (e.g., making drug deals, passing through housing projects, engaging in illicit activities) from completely irrational beliefs (e.g., the perception that the police were standing on the window ledge outside the subject’s 10thfloor apartment). This questionnaire was administered by a rater and typically took 20 minutes for a subject who did not report paranoid experiences and 30-4S minutes for a subject who did. The original development of the Scales of Psychosis Proneness largely followed the sequential steps recommended by Jackson for development of personality scales (22). Items were written and judged according to how well they met trait specifications based on Meehl’s descriptions (23) of these characteristics. Careful attention was given to minimizing bias due to social desirability and acquiescent response style while maximizing item-scale correlations. The Perceptual Aberration Scale is a 35-item truefalse measure of perceptual distortions of body image and of visual and auditory stimuli (13). Analyses of intemnal consistency (13) have revealed alpha values around 0.90 and negligible correlations with age, education, social class, social desirability, and acquiescent response style. Stability of scores, as shown by testretest reliability coefficients, is high (m=0.75) ( 1 7). Representative items (and answers indicating perceptual distortion) include “Parts of my body occasionally seem dead or unreal” (true) and “My hands or feet have never seemed far away” (false). The Magical Ideation Scale is a 30-item true-false scale that measures belief in forms of causation that by conventional standards are invalid (14). Negligible comrelations with age, education, social class, social desimability, and acquiescent response style have been observed (14). Internal consistency has been shown by coefficient alphas in the mid 0.80s, and stability of scores is indicated by test-retest reliability coefficients of 0.80 (17). Representative items (and answers indicating magical ideation) include “Some people can make me aware of them just by thinking of me” (true) and “Homoscopes are might too often for it to be a coincidence” (true). The subjects were given special instructions for filling out the Perceptual Aberration Scale and Magical Ideation Scale. They were told that an occasional item might refer to an experience they had had only when taking drugs and that, unless they had had the experience at other times, they were to mark the item as if they had not had that experience. The mean±SD score on the Perceptual Aberration Scale was 685±7.16, whereas the median was 5.00, in-

1709

COCAINE-INDUCED

dicating

the

PARANOIA

distribution

of

scores

was

positively

skewed. The mean score on the Magical Ideation Scale was 10.15±6.12, and the median was 8.50, so the distribution of scores on this scale was also positively skewed. These group scores were virtually identical to scores on these instruments for large samples of male undergraduate students (Perceptual Aberration Scale= 6.87±6.06; Magical Ideation Scalc=9.73±S.83 ) (L.J. Chapman and J.P. Chapman, unpublished norms, April 1 1, 1989), which showed similarly skewed distnibutions.

As

noted

however,

there

in classifying

by

Lcnzenweger

and

are no established individuals

Loranger

cutoff

among

(20),

scores

for use

nonstudent

clinical

populations as at high and low risk for psychosis. Given the skewed distribution of scores in our group, we fobbowed their strategy of dividing subjects at the group median, which we defined as the total of the median scores on the Perceptual Aberration Scale and Magical Ideation Scale, 13.5. Thus, subjects who scored 13 or lower were considered low risk and those who scored above 1 3 were designated high risk.

RESULTS

Ofthe 10 subjects who reported cocaine-induced pamanoia, eight scored above the combined median score on the Perceptual Aberration Scale and Magical Ideation Scale. Of the 10 who did not report cocaine-rebated pamanoia, only one scored above the median. The strong posi-

nience of cocaine-induced paranoia (or, perhaps, the drug-induced neurobiobogic changes underlying this symptom) increase the likelihood of having perceptual aberrations and magical ideation, or are these latter cxpeniences an expression of an intrinsic vulnerability to paranoia that predated cocaine consumption in otherwise asymptomatic individuals? Chronic cocaine use affects dopaminergic systems (26, 27). These systems have bong been associated with schizophrenia. Thus, cocaine self-administration may represent a quasinaturabistic method for stressing the neurotransmitter system most often linked to the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Some individuals respond to the pharmacologic stress of cocaine by developing transient paranoid symptoms, and it is largely these individuals whose scores on the two scales of psychosis proneness were in the high-risk mange. Perhaps those who develop transient cocaine-induced paranoia possess intrinsic, subclinical vulnerability of the dopaminergic system, a marker of which is endorsement of specific responses on these scales. Although speculative, the concept that dopaminergic dysregulation may underlie forms of both functional and drug-induced paranoia is of considerable heuristic value in the investigation of neurobiological mechanisms of vulnerability It seems reasonable

abs are at high chosis,

to psychosis.

risk

particularly

of

pharmacologic

sity of paranoia and caine binges appear

with

sensitization

size (24),

van-

that

schizophrenic

rate,

significantly

ancc.

It should

for almost

be noted

that

50% in this

of the shared group

the

base

or proportion of individuals who developed cocaine-induced paranoia, was 50.0% (10 of 20) and the selection ratio, or proportion of individuals who were predicted to be psychosis prone, was 45.0% (nine of 20), so the maximum possible phi coefficient was 0.9045. Only ifthe base rate and selection ratio arc equal can a test achieve perfect

than caine. stress

validity (i.e., phi=1.00) ity of the symptom psychosis proneness, ability of not having

this

(25). The of paranoia was 80.0%. cocaine-induced

sensitivity, or probabilgiven the diagnosis of The specificity, or probparanoia given the

absence of psychosis proneness, was predictive powem or probability of given the presence of paranoia, was predictive power, or probability of prone given the absence of paranoia,

90.0%. The positive psychosis proneness 88.9%. The negative not being psychosis was 8 1.8%.

The present who experience port symptoms,

study provides evidence that individuals transient cocaine-induced paranoia reoccurring in the drug-free state, that

are

with

associated

1710

psychosis

proneness.

and correlational, of causality. That

As the it is impossible

is, does

the to

(12,

more

likely

were schizophrenic The relationship of chronic cocaine

quality,

stress.

rapidity increase

when

Indeed,

of its onset oven time,

21 ). Brady

patients

that such individuof prolonged psy-

who

who the the

reported

cocaine

to be of the paranoid patients between exposure,

inten-

during coconsistent

et al. (28) abused

under

the

were

subtype

did not use pharmacologic development

coof

paranoid symptoms, and the onset of psychosis is Unknown. Clearly, larger-scale studies with a longitudinal prospective design are required for full exploration of question

and

for

definitive

determination

whether deviant responses on the proneness scales cede the development of cocaine-induced paranoia persons just beginning long-term cocaine abuse.

of

prein

REFERENCES

DISCUSSION

was cross-sectional determine direction

paranoid

or emotional

tive correlation between cocaine-induced paranoia and psychosis proneness (Fisher’s exact test, p=O.OO3; phi cocfficicnt=0.7035, p

Cocaine-induced paranoia and psychosis proneness.

The aim of this study was to determine whether individuals who experience transient cocaine-induced paranoia are vulnerable to psychosis...
745KB Sizes 0 Downloads 0 Views